Cargando…
Defining filled and empty space: reassessing the filled space illusion for active touch and vision
In the filled space illusion, an extent filled with gratings is estimated as longer than an equivalent extent that is apparently empty. However, researchers do not seem to have carefully considered the terms filled and empty when describing this illusion. Specifically, for active touch, smooth, soli...
Autores principales: | , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
2016
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4978768/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27233286 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4673-x |
_version_ | 1782447216044015616 |
---|---|
author | Collier, Elizabeth S. Lawson, Rebecca |
author_facet | Collier, Elizabeth S. Lawson, Rebecca |
author_sort | Collier, Elizabeth S. |
collection | PubMed |
description | In the filled space illusion, an extent filled with gratings is estimated as longer than an equivalent extent that is apparently empty. However, researchers do not seem to have carefully considered the terms filled and empty when describing this illusion. Specifically, for active touch, smooth, solid surfaces have typically been used to represent empty space. Thus, it is not known whether comparing gratings to truly empty space (air) during active exploration by touch elicits the same illusionary effect. In Experiments 1 and 2, gratings were estimated as longer if they were compared to smooth, solid surfaces rather than being compared to truly empty space. Consistent with this, Experiment 3 showed that empty space was perceived as longer than solid surfaces when the two were compared directly. Together these results are consistent with the hypothesis that, for touch, the standard filled space illusion only occurs if gratings are compared to smooth, solid surfaces and that it may reverse if gratings are compared to empty space. Finally, Experiment 4 showed that gratings were estimated as longer than both solid and empty extents in vision, so the direction of the filled space illusion in vision was not affected by the nature of the comparator. These results are discussed in relation to the dual nature of active touch. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-4978768 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2016 |
publisher | Springer Berlin Heidelberg |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-49787682016-08-18 Defining filled and empty space: reassessing the filled space illusion for active touch and vision Collier, Elizabeth S. Lawson, Rebecca Exp Brain Res Research Article In the filled space illusion, an extent filled with gratings is estimated as longer than an equivalent extent that is apparently empty. However, researchers do not seem to have carefully considered the terms filled and empty when describing this illusion. Specifically, for active touch, smooth, solid surfaces have typically been used to represent empty space. Thus, it is not known whether comparing gratings to truly empty space (air) during active exploration by touch elicits the same illusionary effect. In Experiments 1 and 2, gratings were estimated as longer if they were compared to smooth, solid surfaces rather than being compared to truly empty space. Consistent with this, Experiment 3 showed that empty space was perceived as longer than solid surfaces when the two were compared directly. Together these results are consistent with the hypothesis that, for touch, the standard filled space illusion only occurs if gratings are compared to smooth, solid surfaces and that it may reverse if gratings are compared to empty space. Finally, Experiment 4 showed that gratings were estimated as longer than both solid and empty extents in vision, so the direction of the filled space illusion in vision was not affected by the nature of the comparator. These results are discussed in relation to the dual nature of active touch. Springer Berlin Heidelberg 2016-05-27 2016 /pmc/articles/PMC4978768/ /pubmed/27233286 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4673-x Text en © The Author(s) 2016 Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. |
spellingShingle | Research Article Collier, Elizabeth S. Lawson, Rebecca Defining filled and empty space: reassessing the filled space illusion for active touch and vision |
title | Defining filled and empty space: reassessing the filled space illusion for active touch and vision |
title_full | Defining filled and empty space: reassessing the filled space illusion for active touch and vision |
title_fullStr | Defining filled and empty space: reassessing the filled space illusion for active touch and vision |
title_full_unstemmed | Defining filled and empty space: reassessing the filled space illusion for active touch and vision |
title_short | Defining filled and empty space: reassessing the filled space illusion for active touch and vision |
title_sort | defining filled and empty space: reassessing the filled space illusion for active touch and vision |
topic | Research Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4978768/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27233286 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4673-x |
work_keys_str_mv | AT collierelizabeths definingfilledandemptyspacereassessingthefilledspaceillusionforactivetouchandvision AT lawsonrebecca definingfilledandemptyspacereassessingthefilledspaceillusionforactivetouchandvision |