Cargando…
The reference frame of the tilt aftereffect measured by differential Pavlovian conditioning
We used a differential Pavlovian conditioning paradigm to measure tilt aftereffect (TAE) strength. Gabor patches, rotated clockwise and anticlockwise, were used as conditioned stimuli (CSs), one of which (CS+) was followed by the unconditioned stimulus (UCS), whereas the other (CS−) appeared alone....
Autores principales: | , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Nature Publishing Group
2017
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5240094/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28094321 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep40525 |
_version_ | 1782496004509007872 |
---|---|
author | Nakashima, Yusuke Sugita, Yoichi |
author_facet | Nakashima, Yusuke Sugita, Yoichi |
author_sort | Nakashima, Yusuke |
collection | PubMed |
description | We used a differential Pavlovian conditioning paradigm to measure tilt aftereffect (TAE) strength. Gabor patches, rotated clockwise and anticlockwise, were used as conditioned stimuli (CSs), one of which (CS+) was followed by the unconditioned stimulus (UCS), whereas the other (CS−) appeared alone. The UCS was an air puff delivered to the left eye. In addition to the CS+ and CS−, the vertical test patch was also presented for the clockwise and anticlockwise adapters. The vertical patch was not followed by the UCS. After participants acquired differential conditioning, eyeblink conditioned responses (CRs) were observed for the vertical patch when it appeared to be tilted in the same direction as the CS+ owing to the TAE. The effect was observed not only when the adapter and test stimuli were presented in the same retinotopic position but also when they were presented in the same spatiotopic position, although spatiotopic TAE was weak—it occurred approximately half as often as the full effect. Furthermore, spatiotopic TAE decayed as the time after saccades increased, but did not decay as the time before saccades increased. These results suggest that the time before the performance of saccadic eye movements is needed to compute the spatiotopic representation. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-5240094 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2017 |
publisher | Nature Publishing Group |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-52400942017-01-23 The reference frame of the tilt aftereffect measured by differential Pavlovian conditioning Nakashima, Yusuke Sugita, Yoichi Sci Rep Article We used a differential Pavlovian conditioning paradigm to measure tilt aftereffect (TAE) strength. Gabor patches, rotated clockwise and anticlockwise, were used as conditioned stimuli (CSs), one of which (CS+) was followed by the unconditioned stimulus (UCS), whereas the other (CS−) appeared alone. The UCS was an air puff delivered to the left eye. In addition to the CS+ and CS−, the vertical test patch was also presented for the clockwise and anticlockwise adapters. The vertical patch was not followed by the UCS. After participants acquired differential conditioning, eyeblink conditioned responses (CRs) were observed for the vertical patch when it appeared to be tilted in the same direction as the CS+ owing to the TAE. The effect was observed not only when the adapter and test stimuli were presented in the same retinotopic position but also when they were presented in the same spatiotopic position, although spatiotopic TAE was weak—it occurred approximately half as often as the full effect. Furthermore, spatiotopic TAE decayed as the time after saccades increased, but did not decay as the time before saccades increased. These results suggest that the time before the performance of saccadic eye movements is needed to compute the spatiotopic representation. Nature Publishing Group 2017-01-17 /pmc/articles/PMC5240094/ /pubmed/28094321 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep40525 Text en Copyright © 2017, The Author(s) http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in the credit line; if the material is not included under the Creative Commons license, users will need to obtain permission from the license holder to reproduce the material. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ |
spellingShingle | Article Nakashima, Yusuke Sugita, Yoichi The reference frame of the tilt aftereffect measured by differential Pavlovian conditioning |
title | The reference frame of the tilt aftereffect measured by differential Pavlovian conditioning |
title_full | The reference frame of the tilt aftereffect measured by differential Pavlovian conditioning |
title_fullStr | The reference frame of the tilt aftereffect measured by differential Pavlovian conditioning |
title_full_unstemmed | The reference frame of the tilt aftereffect measured by differential Pavlovian conditioning |
title_short | The reference frame of the tilt aftereffect measured by differential Pavlovian conditioning |
title_sort | reference frame of the tilt aftereffect measured by differential pavlovian conditioning |
topic | Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5240094/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28094321 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep40525 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT nakashimayusuke thereferenceframeofthetiltaftereffectmeasuredbydifferentialpavlovianconditioning AT sugitayoichi thereferenceframeofthetiltaftereffectmeasuredbydifferentialpavlovianconditioning AT nakashimayusuke referenceframeofthetiltaftereffectmeasuredbydifferentialpavlovianconditioning AT sugitayoichi referenceframeofthetiltaftereffectmeasuredbydifferentialpavlovianconditioning |