Cargando…

Talking to your car can drive you to distraction

This research examined the impact of in-vehicle information system (IVIS) interactions on the driver’s cognitive workload; 257 subjects participated in a weeklong evaluation of the IVIS interaction in one of ten different model-year 2015 automobiles. After an initial assessment of the cognitive work...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Strayer, David L., Cooper, Joel M., Turrill, Jonna, Coleman, James R., Hopman, Rachel J.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Springer International Publishing 2016
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5256443/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28180167
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s41235-016-0018-3
_version_ 1782498715373666304
author Strayer, David L.
Cooper, Joel M.
Turrill, Jonna
Coleman, James R.
Hopman, Rachel J.
author_facet Strayer, David L.
Cooper, Joel M.
Turrill, Jonna
Coleman, James R.
Hopman, Rachel J.
author_sort Strayer, David L.
collection PubMed
description This research examined the impact of in-vehicle information system (IVIS) interactions on the driver’s cognitive workload; 257 subjects participated in a weeklong evaluation of the IVIS interaction in one of ten different model-year 2015 automobiles. After an initial assessment of the cognitive workload associated with using the IVIS, participants took the vehicle home for 5 days and practiced using the system. At the end of the 5 days of practice, participants returned and the workload of these IVIS interactions was reassessed. The cognitive workload was found to be moderate to high, averaging 3.34 on a 5-point scale and ranged from 2.37 to 4.57. The workload was associated with the intuitiveness and complexity of the system and the time it took participants to complete the interaction. The workload experienced by older drivers was significantly greater than that experienced by younger drivers performing the same operations. Practice did not eliminate the interference from IVIS interactions. In fact, IVIS interactions that were difficult on the first day were still relatively difficult to perform after a week of practice. Finally, there were long-lasting residual costs after the IVIS interactions had terminated. The higher levels of workload should serve as a caution that these voice-based interactions can be cognitively demanding and ought not to be used indiscriminately while operating a motor vehicle.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-5256443
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2016
publisher Springer International Publishing
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-52564432017-02-06 Talking to your car can drive you to distraction Strayer, David L. Cooper, Joel M. Turrill, Jonna Coleman, James R. Hopman, Rachel J. Cogn Res Princ Implic Original Article This research examined the impact of in-vehicle information system (IVIS) interactions on the driver’s cognitive workload; 257 subjects participated in a weeklong evaluation of the IVIS interaction in one of ten different model-year 2015 automobiles. After an initial assessment of the cognitive workload associated with using the IVIS, participants took the vehicle home for 5 days and practiced using the system. At the end of the 5 days of practice, participants returned and the workload of these IVIS interactions was reassessed. The cognitive workload was found to be moderate to high, averaging 3.34 on a 5-point scale and ranged from 2.37 to 4.57. The workload was associated with the intuitiveness and complexity of the system and the time it took participants to complete the interaction. The workload experienced by older drivers was significantly greater than that experienced by younger drivers performing the same operations. Practice did not eliminate the interference from IVIS interactions. In fact, IVIS interactions that were difficult on the first day were still relatively difficult to perform after a week of practice. Finally, there were long-lasting residual costs after the IVIS interactions had terminated. The higher levels of workload should serve as a caution that these voice-based interactions can be cognitively demanding and ought not to be used indiscriminately while operating a motor vehicle. Springer International Publishing 2016-11-14 /pmc/articles/PMC5256443/ /pubmed/28180167 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s41235-016-0018-3 Text en © The Author(s) 2016 Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
spellingShingle Original Article
Strayer, David L.
Cooper, Joel M.
Turrill, Jonna
Coleman, James R.
Hopman, Rachel J.
Talking to your car can drive you to distraction
title Talking to your car can drive you to distraction
title_full Talking to your car can drive you to distraction
title_fullStr Talking to your car can drive you to distraction
title_full_unstemmed Talking to your car can drive you to distraction
title_short Talking to your car can drive you to distraction
title_sort talking to your car can drive you to distraction
topic Original Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5256443/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28180167
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s41235-016-0018-3
work_keys_str_mv AT strayerdavidl talkingtoyourcarcandriveyoutodistraction
AT cooperjoelm talkingtoyourcarcandriveyoutodistraction
AT turrilljonna talkingtoyourcarcandriveyoutodistraction
AT colemanjamesr talkingtoyourcarcandriveyoutodistraction
AT hopmanrachelj talkingtoyourcarcandriveyoutodistraction