Cargando…

Vector and position coding in goal-directed movements

Two different ways to code a goal-directed movement have been proposed in the literature: vector coding and position coding. Assuming that the code is fine-tuned if a movement is immediately repeated, one can predict that repeating movements to the same endpoint will increase precision if movements...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: van der Graaff, Marieke C. W., Brenner, Eli, Smeets, Jeroen B. J.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Springer Berlin Heidelberg 2016
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5315739/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27858127
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4828-9
_version_ 1782508716871909376
author van der Graaff, Marieke C. W.
Brenner, Eli
Smeets, Jeroen B. J.
author_facet van der Graaff, Marieke C. W.
Brenner, Eli
Smeets, Jeroen B. J.
author_sort van der Graaff, Marieke C. W.
collection PubMed
description Two different ways to code a goal-directed movement have been proposed in the literature: vector coding and position coding. Assuming that the code is fine-tuned if a movement is immediately repeated, one can predict that repeating movements to the same endpoint will increase precision if movements are coded in terms of the position of the endpoint. Repeating the same movement vector at slightly different positions will increase precision if movements are coded in terms of vectors. Following this reasoning, Hudson and Landy (J Neurophys 108(10):2708–2716, 2012) found evidence for both types of coding when participants moved their hand over a table while the target and feedback were provided on a vertical screen. Do we also see evidence for both types of coding if participants repeat movements within a more natural visuo-motor mapping? To find out, we repeated the study of Hudson and Landy (J Neurophys 108(10):2708–2716, 2012), but our participants made movements directly to the targets. We compared the same movements embedded in blocks of repetitions of endpoints and blocks of repetitions of movement vectors. Within blocks, the movements were presented in a random order. We found no benefit of repeating either a position or a vector. We subsequently repeated the experiment with a similar mapping between movements and images to those used by Hudson and Landy and found that participants only clearly benefit from repeating a position. We conclude that repeating a position is particularly useful when dealing with unusual visuo-motor mappings.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-5315739
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2016
publisher Springer Berlin Heidelberg
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-53157392017-03-02 Vector and position coding in goal-directed movements van der Graaff, Marieke C. W. Brenner, Eli Smeets, Jeroen B. J. Exp Brain Res Research Article Two different ways to code a goal-directed movement have been proposed in the literature: vector coding and position coding. Assuming that the code is fine-tuned if a movement is immediately repeated, one can predict that repeating movements to the same endpoint will increase precision if movements are coded in terms of the position of the endpoint. Repeating the same movement vector at slightly different positions will increase precision if movements are coded in terms of vectors. Following this reasoning, Hudson and Landy (J Neurophys 108(10):2708–2716, 2012) found evidence for both types of coding when participants moved their hand over a table while the target and feedback were provided on a vertical screen. Do we also see evidence for both types of coding if participants repeat movements within a more natural visuo-motor mapping? To find out, we repeated the study of Hudson and Landy (J Neurophys 108(10):2708–2716, 2012), but our participants made movements directly to the targets. We compared the same movements embedded in blocks of repetitions of endpoints and blocks of repetitions of movement vectors. Within blocks, the movements were presented in a random order. We found no benefit of repeating either a position or a vector. We subsequently repeated the experiment with a similar mapping between movements and images to those used by Hudson and Landy and found that participants only clearly benefit from repeating a position. We conclude that repeating a position is particularly useful when dealing with unusual visuo-motor mappings. Springer Berlin Heidelberg 2016-11-17 2017 /pmc/articles/PMC5315739/ /pubmed/27858127 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4828-9 Text en © The Author(s) 2016 Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
spellingShingle Research Article
van der Graaff, Marieke C. W.
Brenner, Eli
Smeets, Jeroen B. J.
Vector and position coding in goal-directed movements
title Vector and position coding in goal-directed movements
title_full Vector and position coding in goal-directed movements
title_fullStr Vector and position coding in goal-directed movements
title_full_unstemmed Vector and position coding in goal-directed movements
title_short Vector and position coding in goal-directed movements
title_sort vector and position coding in goal-directed movements
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5315739/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27858127
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4828-9
work_keys_str_mv AT vandergraaffmariekecw vectorandpositioncodingingoaldirectedmovements
AT brennereli vectorandpositioncodingingoaldirectedmovements
AT smeetsjeroenbj vectorandpositioncodingingoaldirectedmovements