Cargando…
Age-Dependent Changes of Thinking about Verbs
We investigated the knowledge of emotional and motor verbs in children and adolescents from three age ranges (8–11, 12–15, 16–19 years). Participants estimated the verbs familiarity, age of acquisition, valence, arousal, imageability, and motor- and emotion-relatedness. Participants were familiar wi...
Autores principales: | , , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2017
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5348498/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28352219 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00040 |
_version_ | 1782514240966361088 |
---|---|
author | Bonivento, Carolina Tomasino, Barbara Garzitto, Marco Piccin, Sara Fabbro, Franco Brambilla, Paolo |
author_facet | Bonivento, Carolina Tomasino, Barbara Garzitto, Marco Piccin, Sara Fabbro, Franco Brambilla, Paolo |
author_sort | Bonivento, Carolina |
collection | PubMed |
description | We investigated the knowledge of emotional and motor verbs in children and adolescents from three age ranges (8–11, 12–15, 16–19 years). Participants estimated the verbs familiarity, age of acquisition, valence, arousal, imageability, and motor- and emotion-relatedness. Participants were familiar with the verbs in our dataset. The younger (8–11) attributed an emotional character to the verbs less frequently than the middle (12–15) and the older (16–19) groups. In the 8–11 group males rated the verbs as emotion-related less frequently than females. Results indicate that processing verbal concepts as emotion-related develops gradually, and after 12–15 is rather stable. The age of acquisition (AoA) develops late: the older (16–19) had a higher awareness in reporting that they learnt the verbs earlier as compared to the estimations made by the younger (8–11 and 12–15). AoA positively correlated with attribution of emotion relatedness meaning that emotion-related verbs were learned later. Arousal was comparable across ages. Also it increased when attributing motor relatedness to verbs and decreased when attributing emotion relatedness. Reporting the verbs' affective valence (happy vs. unhappy) changes with age: younger (8–11) judged the verbs generally more “happy” than both the older groups. Instead the middle and the older group did not show differences. Happiness increased when processing the verbs as motor related and decreased when processing the verbs as emotion related. Age affected imageability: the younger (8–11) considered the verbs easier to be imagined than the two older groups, suggesting that at this age vividness estimation is still rough, while after 12–15 is stable as the 12–15 and 15–19 group did not differ. Imageability predicted arousal, AoA, emotion- and motor-relatedness indicating that this index influences the way verbs are processed. Imageability was positively correlated to emotion relatedness, indicating that such verbs were harder to be imagined, and negatively to motor relatedness. Imageablity positively correlated with valence meaning that verbs receiving positive valence were also those that were hard to be imagined, and negatively correlated with arousal, meaning that verbs that were harder to be imagined elicited low physiological activation. Our results give an insight in the development of emotional and motor-related verbs representations. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-5348498 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2017 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-53484982017-03-28 Age-Dependent Changes of Thinking about Verbs Bonivento, Carolina Tomasino, Barbara Garzitto, Marco Piccin, Sara Fabbro, Franco Brambilla, Paolo Front Behav Neurosci Neuroscience We investigated the knowledge of emotional and motor verbs in children and adolescents from three age ranges (8–11, 12–15, 16–19 years). Participants estimated the verbs familiarity, age of acquisition, valence, arousal, imageability, and motor- and emotion-relatedness. Participants were familiar with the verbs in our dataset. The younger (8–11) attributed an emotional character to the verbs less frequently than the middle (12–15) and the older (16–19) groups. In the 8–11 group males rated the verbs as emotion-related less frequently than females. Results indicate that processing verbal concepts as emotion-related develops gradually, and after 12–15 is rather stable. The age of acquisition (AoA) develops late: the older (16–19) had a higher awareness in reporting that they learnt the verbs earlier as compared to the estimations made by the younger (8–11 and 12–15). AoA positively correlated with attribution of emotion relatedness meaning that emotion-related verbs were learned later. Arousal was comparable across ages. Also it increased when attributing motor relatedness to verbs and decreased when attributing emotion relatedness. Reporting the verbs' affective valence (happy vs. unhappy) changes with age: younger (8–11) judged the verbs generally more “happy” than both the older groups. Instead the middle and the older group did not show differences. Happiness increased when processing the verbs as motor related and decreased when processing the verbs as emotion related. Age affected imageability: the younger (8–11) considered the verbs easier to be imagined than the two older groups, suggesting that at this age vividness estimation is still rough, while after 12–15 is stable as the 12–15 and 15–19 group did not differ. Imageability predicted arousal, AoA, emotion- and motor-relatedness indicating that this index influences the way verbs are processed. Imageability was positively correlated to emotion relatedness, indicating that such verbs were harder to be imagined, and negatively to motor relatedness. Imageablity positively correlated with valence meaning that verbs receiving positive valence were also those that were hard to be imagined, and negatively correlated with arousal, meaning that verbs that were harder to be imagined elicited low physiological activation. Our results give an insight in the development of emotional and motor-related verbs representations. Frontiers Media S.A. 2017-03-14 /pmc/articles/PMC5348498/ /pubmed/28352219 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00040 Text en Copyright © 2017 Bonivento, Tomasino, Garzitto, Piccin, Fabbro and Brambilla. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Neuroscience Bonivento, Carolina Tomasino, Barbara Garzitto, Marco Piccin, Sara Fabbro, Franco Brambilla, Paolo Age-Dependent Changes of Thinking about Verbs |
title | Age-Dependent Changes of Thinking about Verbs |
title_full | Age-Dependent Changes of Thinking about Verbs |
title_fullStr | Age-Dependent Changes of Thinking about Verbs |
title_full_unstemmed | Age-Dependent Changes of Thinking about Verbs |
title_short | Age-Dependent Changes of Thinking about Verbs |
title_sort | age-dependent changes of thinking about verbs |
topic | Neuroscience |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5348498/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28352219 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00040 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT boniventocarolina agedependentchangesofthinkingaboutverbs AT tomasinobarbara agedependentchangesofthinkingaboutverbs AT garzittomarco agedependentchangesofthinkingaboutverbs AT piccinsara agedependentchangesofthinkingaboutverbs AT fabbrofranco agedependentchangesofthinkingaboutverbs AT brambillapaolo agedependentchangesofthinkingaboutverbs |