Cargando…

Who Believes in the Storybook Image of the Scientist?

Do lay people and scientists themselves recognize that scientists are human and therefore prone to human fallibilities such as error, bias, and even dishonesty? In a series of three experimental studies and one correlational study (total N = 3,278) we found that the “storybook image of the scientist...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Veldkamp, Coosje L. S., Hartgerink, Chris H. J., van Assen, Marcel A. L. M., Wicherts, Jelte M.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Taylor & Francis 2017
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5359741/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28001440
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08989621.2016.1268922
_version_ 1782516436603764736
author Veldkamp, Coosje L. S.
Hartgerink, Chris H. J.
van Assen, Marcel A. L. M.
Wicherts, Jelte M.
author_facet Veldkamp, Coosje L. S.
Hartgerink, Chris H. J.
van Assen, Marcel A. L. M.
Wicherts, Jelte M.
author_sort Veldkamp, Coosje L. S.
collection PubMed
description Do lay people and scientists themselves recognize that scientists are human and therefore prone to human fallibilities such as error, bias, and even dishonesty? In a series of three experimental studies and one correlational study (total N = 3,278) we found that the “storybook image of the scientist” is pervasive: American lay people and scientists from over 60 countries attributed considerably more objectivity, rationality, open-mindedness, intelligence, integrity, and communality to scientists than to other highly-educated people. Moreover, scientists perceived even larger differences than lay people did. Some groups of scientists also differentiated between different categories of scientists: established scientists attributed higher levels of the scientific traits to established scientists than to early-career scientists and Ph.D. students, and higher levels to Ph.D. students than to early-career scientists. Female scientists attributed considerably higher levels of the scientific traits to female scientists than to male scientists. A strong belief in the storybook image and the (human) tendency to attribute higher levels of desirable traits to people in one’s own group than to people in other groups may decrease scientists’ willingness to adopt recently proposed practices to reduce error, bias and dishonesty in science.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-5359741
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2017
publisher Taylor & Francis
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-53597412017-04-05 Who Believes in the Storybook Image of the Scientist? Veldkamp, Coosje L. S. Hartgerink, Chris H. J. van Assen, Marcel A. L. M. Wicherts, Jelte M. Account Res Original Articles Do lay people and scientists themselves recognize that scientists are human and therefore prone to human fallibilities such as error, bias, and even dishonesty? In a series of three experimental studies and one correlational study (total N = 3,278) we found that the “storybook image of the scientist” is pervasive: American lay people and scientists from over 60 countries attributed considerably more objectivity, rationality, open-mindedness, intelligence, integrity, and communality to scientists than to other highly-educated people. Moreover, scientists perceived even larger differences than lay people did. Some groups of scientists also differentiated between different categories of scientists: established scientists attributed higher levels of the scientific traits to established scientists than to early-career scientists and Ph.D. students, and higher levels to Ph.D. students than to early-career scientists. Female scientists attributed considerably higher levels of the scientific traits to female scientists than to male scientists. A strong belief in the storybook image and the (human) tendency to attribute higher levels of desirable traits to people in one’s own group than to people in other groups may decrease scientists’ willingness to adopt recently proposed practices to reduce error, bias and dishonesty in science. Taylor & Francis 2017-04-03 2016-12-21 /pmc/articles/PMC5359741/ /pubmed/28001440 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08989621.2016.1268922 Text en © 2015 The Author(s). Published by Taylor & Francis. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
spellingShingle Original Articles
Veldkamp, Coosje L. S.
Hartgerink, Chris H. J.
van Assen, Marcel A. L. M.
Wicherts, Jelte M.
Who Believes in the Storybook Image of the Scientist?
title Who Believes in the Storybook Image of the Scientist?
title_full Who Believes in the Storybook Image of the Scientist?
title_fullStr Who Believes in the Storybook Image of the Scientist?
title_full_unstemmed Who Believes in the Storybook Image of the Scientist?
title_short Who Believes in the Storybook Image of the Scientist?
title_sort who believes in the storybook image of the scientist?
topic Original Articles
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5359741/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28001440
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08989621.2016.1268922
work_keys_str_mv AT veldkampcoosjels whobelievesinthestorybookimageofthescientist
AT hartgerinkchrishj whobelievesinthestorybookimageofthescientist
AT vanassenmarcelalm whobelievesinthestorybookimageofthescientist
AT wichertsjeltem whobelievesinthestorybookimageofthescientist