Cargando…

The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground

Interlocutors converge on names to refer to entities. For example, a speaker might refer to a novel looking object as the jellyfish and, once identified, the listener will too. The hypothesized mechanism behind such referential precedents is a subject of debate. The common ground view claims that li...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Kronmüller, Edmundo, Noveck, Ira, Rivera, Natalia, Jaume-Guazzini, Francisco, Barr, Dale
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: The Royal Society Publishing 2017
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5367312/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28386440
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160827
_version_ 1782517749668380672
author Kronmüller, Edmundo
Noveck, Ira
Rivera, Natalia
Jaume-Guazzini, Francisco
Barr, Dale
author_facet Kronmüller, Edmundo
Noveck, Ira
Rivera, Natalia
Jaume-Guazzini, Francisco
Barr, Dale
author_sort Kronmüller, Edmundo
collection PubMed
description Interlocutors converge on names to refer to entities. For example, a speaker might refer to a novel looking object as the jellyfish and, once identified, the listener will too. The hypothesized mechanism behind such referential precedents is a subject of debate. The common ground view claims that listeners register the object as well as the identity of the speaker who coined the label. The linguistic view claims that, once established, precedents are treated by listeners like any other linguistic unit, i.e. without needing to keep track of the speaker. To test predictions from each account, we used visual-world eyetracking, which allows observations in real time, during a standard referential communication task. Participants had to select objects based on instructions from two speakers. In the critical condition, listeners sought an object with a negative reference such as not the jellyfish. We aimed to determine the extent to which listeners rely on the linguistic input, common ground or both. We found that initial interpretations were based on linguistic processing only and that common ground considerations do emerge but only after 1000 ms. Our findings support the idea that—at least temporally—linguistic processing can be isolated from common ground.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-5367312
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2017
publisher The Royal Society Publishing
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-53673122017-04-06 The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground Kronmüller, Edmundo Noveck, Ira Rivera, Natalia Jaume-Guazzini, Francisco Barr, Dale R Soc Open Sci Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience Interlocutors converge on names to refer to entities. For example, a speaker might refer to a novel looking object as the jellyfish and, once identified, the listener will too. The hypothesized mechanism behind such referential precedents is a subject of debate. The common ground view claims that listeners register the object as well as the identity of the speaker who coined the label. The linguistic view claims that, once established, precedents are treated by listeners like any other linguistic unit, i.e. without needing to keep track of the speaker. To test predictions from each account, we used visual-world eyetracking, which allows observations in real time, during a standard referential communication task. Participants had to select objects based on instructions from two speakers. In the critical condition, listeners sought an object with a negative reference such as not the jellyfish. We aimed to determine the extent to which listeners rely on the linguistic input, common ground or both. We found that initial interpretations were based on linguistic processing only and that common ground considerations do emerge but only after 1000 ms. Our findings support the idea that—at least temporally—linguistic processing can be isolated from common ground. The Royal Society Publishing 2017-02-08 /pmc/articles/PMC5367312/ /pubmed/28386440 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160827 Text en © 2017 The Authors. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ Published by the Royal Society under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/, which permits unrestricted use, provided the original author and source are credited.
spellingShingle Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience
Kronmüller, Edmundo
Noveck, Ira
Rivera, Natalia
Jaume-Guazzini, Francisco
Barr, Dale
The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground
title The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground
title_full The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground
title_fullStr The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground
title_full_unstemmed The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground
title_short The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground
title_sort positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground
topic Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5367312/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28386440
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160827
work_keys_str_mv AT kronmulleredmundo thepositivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground
AT noveckira thepositivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground
AT riveranatalia thepositivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground
AT jaumeguazzinifrancisco thepositivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground
AT barrdale thepositivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground
AT kronmulleredmundo positivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground
AT noveckira positivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground
AT riveranatalia positivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground
AT jaumeguazzinifrancisco positivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground
AT barrdale positivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground