Cargando…
The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground
Interlocutors converge on names to refer to entities. For example, a speaker might refer to a novel looking object as the jellyfish and, once identified, the listener will too. The hypothesized mechanism behind such referential precedents is a subject of debate. The common ground view claims that li...
Autores principales: | , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
The Royal Society Publishing
2017
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5367312/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28386440 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160827 |
_version_ | 1782517749668380672 |
---|---|
author | Kronmüller, Edmundo Noveck, Ira Rivera, Natalia Jaume-Guazzini, Francisco Barr, Dale |
author_facet | Kronmüller, Edmundo Noveck, Ira Rivera, Natalia Jaume-Guazzini, Francisco Barr, Dale |
author_sort | Kronmüller, Edmundo |
collection | PubMed |
description | Interlocutors converge on names to refer to entities. For example, a speaker might refer to a novel looking object as the jellyfish and, once identified, the listener will too. The hypothesized mechanism behind such referential precedents is a subject of debate. The common ground view claims that listeners register the object as well as the identity of the speaker who coined the label. The linguistic view claims that, once established, precedents are treated by listeners like any other linguistic unit, i.e. without needing to keep track of the speaker. To test predictions from each account, we used visual-world eyetracking, which allows observations in real time, during a standard referential communication task. Participants had to select objects based on instructions from two speakers. In the critical condition, listeners sought an object with a negative reference such as not the jellyfish. We aimed to determine the extent to which listeners rely on the linguistic input, common ground or both. We found that initial interpretations were based on linguistic processing only and that common ground considerations do emerge but only after 1000 ms. Our findings support the idea that—at least temporally—linguistic processing can be isolated from common ground. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-5367312 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2017 |
publisher | The Royal Society Publishing |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-53673122017-04-06 The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground Kronmüller, Edmundo Noveck, Ira Rivera, Natalia Jaume-Guazzini, Francisco Barr, Dale R Soc Open Sci Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience Interlocutors converge on names to refer to entities. For example, a speaker might refer to a novel looking object as the jellyfish and, once identified, the listener will too. The hypothesized mechanism behind such referential precedents is a subject of debate. The common ground view claims that listeners register the object as well as the identity of the speaker who coined the label. The linguistic view claims that, once established, precedents are treated by listeners like any other linguistic unit, i.e. without needing to keep track of the speaker. To test predictions from each account, we used visual-world eyetracking, which allows observations in real time, during a standard referential communication task. Participants had to select objects based on instructions from two speakers. In the critical condition, listeners sought an object with a negative reference such as not the jellyfish. We aimed to determine the extent to which listeners rely on the linguistic input, common ground or both. We found that initial interpretations were based on linguistic processing only and that common ground considerations do emerge but only after 1000 ms. Our findings support the idea that—at least temporally—linguistic processing can be isolated from common ground. The Royal Society Publishing 2017-02-08 /pmc/articles/PMC5367312/ /pubmed/28386440 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160827 Text en © 2017 The Authors. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ Published by the Royal Society under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/, which permits unrestricted use, provided the original author and source are credited. |
spellingShingle | Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience Kronmüller, Edmundo Noveck, Ira Rivera, Natalia Jaume-Guazzini, Francisco Barr, Dale The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground |
title | The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground |
title_full | The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground |
title_fullStr | The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground |
title_full_unstemmed | The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground |
title_short | The positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground |
title_sort | positive side of a negative reference: the delay between linguistic processing and common ground |
topic | Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5367312/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28386440 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160827 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT kronmulleredmundo thepositivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground AT noveckira thepositivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground AT riveranatalia thepositivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground AT jaumeguazzinifrancisco thepositivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground AT barrdale thepositivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground AT kronmulleredmundo positivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground AT noveckira positivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground AT riveranatalia positivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground AT jaumeguazzinifrancisco positivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground AT barrdale positivesideofanegativereferencethedelaybetweenlinguisticprocessingandcommonground |