Cargando…

Visuo‐manual tracking: does intermittent control with aperiodic sampling explain linear power and non‐linear remnant without sensorimotor noise?

KEY POINTS: A human controlling an external system is described most easily and conventionally as linearly and continuously translating sensory input to motor output, with the inevitable output remnant, non‐linearly related to the input, attributed to sensorimotor noise. Recent experiments show sust...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Gollee, Henrik, Gawthrop, Peter J., Lakie, Martin, Loram, Ian D.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: John Wiley and Sons Inc. 2017
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5663819/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28833126
http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/JP274288
_version_ 1783274887514685440
author Gollee, Henrik
Gawthrop, Peter J.
Lakie, Martin
Loram, Ian D.
author_facet Gollee, Henrik
Gawthrop, Peter J.
Lakie, Martin
Loram, Ian D.
author_sort Gollee, Henrik
collection PubMed
description KEY POINTS: A human controlling an external system is described most easily and conventionally as linearly and continuously translating sensory input to motor output, with the inevitable output remnant, non‐linearly related to the input, attributed to sensorimotor noise. Recent experiments show sustained manual tracking involves repeated refractoriness (insensitivity to sensory information for a certain duration), with the temporary 200–500 ms periods of irresponsiveness to sensory input making the control process intrinsically non‐linear. This evidence calls for re‐examination of the extent to which random sensorimotor noise is required to explain the non‐linear remnant. This investigation of manual tracking shows how the full motor output (linear component and remnant) can be explained mechanistically by aperiodic sampling triggered by prediction error thresholds. Whereas broadband physiological noise is general to all processes, aperiodic sampling is associated with sensorimotor decision making within specific frontal, striatal and parietal networks; we conclude that manual tracking utilises such slow serial decision making pathways up to several times per second. ABSTRACT: The human operator is described adequately by linear translation of sensory input to motor output. Motor output also always includes a non‐linear remnant resulting from random sensorimotor noise from multiple sources, and non‐linear input transformations, for example thresholds or refractory periods. Recent evidence showed that manual tracking incurs substantial, serial, refractoriness (insensitivity to sensory information of 350 and 550 ms for 1st and 2nd order systems respectively). Our two questions are: (i) What are the comparative merits of explaining the non‐linear remnant using noise or non‐linear transformations? (ii) Can non‐linear transformations represent serial motor decision making within the sensorimotor feedback loop intrinsic to tracking? Twelve participants (instructed to act in three prescribed ways) manually controlled two systems (1st and 2nd order) subject to a periodic multi‐sine disturbance. Joystick power was analysed using three models, continuous‐linear‐control (CC), continuous‐linear‐control with calculated noise spectrum (CCN), and intermittent control with aperiodic sampling triggered by prediction error thresholds (IC). Unlike the linear mechanism, the intermittent control mechanism explained the majority of total power (linear and remnant) (77–87% vs. 8–48%, IC vs. CC). Between conditions, IC used thresholds and distributions of open loop intervals consistent with, respectively, instructions and previous measured, model independent values; whereas CCN required changes in noise spectrum deviating from broadband, signal dependent noise. We conclude that manual tracking uses open loop predictive control with aperiodic sampling. Because aperiodic sampling is inherent to serial decision making within previously identified, specific frontal, striatal and parietal networks we suggest that these structures are intimately involved in visuo‐manual tracking.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-5663819
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2017
publisher John Wiley and Sons Inc.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-56638192017-11-08 Visuo‐manual tracking: does intermittent control with aperiodic sampling explain linear power and non‐linear remnant without sensorimotor noise? Gollee, Henrik Gawthrop, Peter J. Lakie, Martin Loram, Ian D. J Physiol Neuroscience ‐ Behavioural/Systems/Cognitive KEY POINTS: A human controlling an external system is described most easily and conventionally as linearly and continuously translating sensory input to motor output, with the inevitable output remnant, non‐linearly related to the input, attributed to sensorimotor noise. Recent experiments show sustained manual tracking involves repeated refractoriness (insensitivity to sensory information for a certain duration), with the temporary 200–500 ms periods of irresponsiveness to sensory input making the control process intrinsically non‐linear. This evidence calls for re‐examination of the extent to which random sensorimotor noise is required to explain the non‐linear remnant. This investigation of manual tracking shows how the full motor output (linear component and remnant) can be explained mechanistically by aperiodic sampling triggered by prediction error thresholds. Whereas broadband physiological noise is general to all processes, aperiodic sampling is associated with sensorimotor decision making within specific frontal, striatal and parietal networks; we conclude that manual tracking utilises such slow serial decision making pathways up to several times per second. ABSTRACT: The human operator is described adequately by linear translation of sensory input to motor output. Motor output also always includes a non‐linear remnant resulting from random sensorimotor noise from multiple sources, and non‐linear input transformations, for example thresholds or refractory periods. Recent evidence showed that manual tracking incurs substantial, serial, refractoriness (insensitivity to sensory information of 350 and 550 ms for 1st and 2nd order systems respectively). Our two questions are: (i) What are the comparative merits of explaining the non‐linear remnant using noise or non‐linear transformations? (ii) Can non‐linear transformations represent serial motor decision making within the sensorimotor feedback loop intrinsic to tracking? Twelve participants (instructed to act in three prescribed ways) manually controlled two systems (1st and 2nd order) subject to a periodic multi‐sine disturbance. Joystick power was analysed using three models, continuous‐linear‐control (CC), continuous‐linear‐control with calculated noise spectrum (CCN), and intermittent control with aperiodic sampling triggered by prediction error thresholds (IC). Unlike the linear mechanism, the intermittent control mechanism explained the majority of total power (linear and remnant) (77–87% vs. 8–48%, IC vs. CC). Between conditions, IC used thresholds and distributions of open loop intervals consistent with, respectively, instructions and previous measured, model independent values; whereas CCN required changes in noise spectrum deviating from broadband, signal dependent noise. We conclude that manual tracking uses open loop predictive control with aperiodic sampling. Because aperiodic sampling is inherent to serial decision making within previously identified, specific frontal, striatal and parietal networks we suggest that these structures are intimately involved in visuo‐manual tracking. John Wiley and Sons Inc. 2017-10-01 2017-11-01 /pmc/articles/PMC5663819/ /pubmed/28833126 http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/JP274288 Text en © 2017 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of The Physiological Society This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
spellingShingle Neuroscience ‐ Behavioural/Systems/Cognitive
Gollee, Henrik
Gawthrop, Peter J.
Lakie, Martin
Loram, Ian D.
Visuo‐manual tracking: does intermittent control with aperiodic sampling explain linear power and non‐linear remnant without sensorimotor noise?
title Visuo‐manual tracking: does intermittent control with aperiodic sampling explain linear power and non‐linear remnant without sensorimotor noise?
title_full Visuo‐manual tracking: does intermittent control with aperiodic sampling explain linear power and non‐linear remnant without sensorimotor noise?
title_fullStr Visuo‐manual tracking: does intermittent control with aperiodic sampling explain linear power and non‐linear remnant without sensorimotor noise?
title_full_unstemmed Visuo‐manual tracking: does intermittent control with aperiodic sampling explain linear power and non‐linear remnant without sensorimotor noise?
title_short Visuo‐manual tracking: does intermittent control with aperiodic sampling explain linear power and non‐linear remnant without sensorimotor noise?
title_sort visuo‐manual tracking: does intermittent control with aperiodic sampling explain linear power and non‐linear remnant without sensorimotor noise?
topic Neuroscience ‐ Behavioural/Systems/Cognitive
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5663819/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28833126
http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/JP274288
work_keys_str_mv AT golleehenrik visuomanualtrackingdoesintermittentcontrolwithaperiodicsamplingexplainlinearpowerandnonlinearremnantwithoutsensorimotornoise
AT gawthroppeterj visuomanualtrackingdoesintermittentcontrolwithaperiodicsamplingexplainlinearpowerandnonlinearremnantwithoutsensorimotornoise
AT lakiemartin visuomanualtrackingdoesintermittentcontrolwithaperiodicsamplingexplainlinearpowerandnonlinearremnantwithoutsensorimotornoise
AT loramiand visuomanualtrackingdoesintermittentcontrolwithaperiodicsamplingexplainlinearpowerandnonlinearremnantwithoutsensorimotornoise