Cargando…

Express saccades in distinct populations: east, west, and in-between

Express saccades are low latency (80–130 ms), visually guided saccades. While their occurrence is encouraged by the use of gap tasks (the fixation target is extinguished 200 ms prior to the saccade target appearing) and suppressed by the use of overlap tasks (the fixation target remains present when...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Knox, Paul C., Wolohan, Felicity D. A., Helmy, Mai S.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Springer Berlin Heidelberg 2017
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5671528/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28956092
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5094-1
_version_ 1783276251326185472
author Knox, Paul C.
Wolohan, Felicity D. A.
Helmy, Mai S.
author_facet Knox, Paul C.
Wolohan, Felicity D. A.
Helmy, Mai S.
author_sort Knox, Paul C.
collection PubMed
description Express saccades are low latency (80–130 ms), visually guided saccades. While their occurrence is encouraged by the use of gap tasks (the fixation target is extinguished 200 ms prior to the saccade target appearing) and suppressed by the use of overlap tasks (the fixation target remains present when the saccade target appears), there are some healthy, adult participants, “express saccade makers” (ESMs), who persist in generating high proportions (> 30%) of express saccades in overlap conditions. These participants are encountered much more frequently in Chinese participant groups than amongst the Caucasian participants tested to date. What is not known is whether this high number of ESMs is only a feature of Chinese participant groups. More broadly, there are few comparative studies of saccade behaviour across large participant groups drawn from different populations. We, therefore, tested an independent group of 70 healthy adult Egyptian participants, using the same equipment and procedures as employed in the previous studies. Each participant was exposed to two blocks of 200 gap, and two blocks of 200 overlap trials, with block order counterbalanced. Results from the Schwartz Value Survey were used to confirm that this group of participants was culturally distinct from the Chinese and Caucasian (white British) groups tested previously. Fourteen percent (10/70) of this new group were ESMs, and the pattern of latency distribution in these ESMs was identical to that identified in the other participant groups, with a prominent peak in the express latency range in overlap conditions. Overall, we identified three modes in the distribution of saccade latency in overlap conditions, the timing of which (express peak at 110 ms, subsequent peaks at 160 and 210 ms) were strikingly consistent with our previous observations. That these behavioural patterns of saccade latency are observed consistently in large participant groups, drawn from geographically, ethnically, and culturally distinct populations, suggests that they relate to the underlying architecture of the saccade system. ELECTRONIC SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL: The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s00221-017-5094-1) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-5671528
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2017
publisher Springer Berlin Heidelberg
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-56715282017-11-17 Express saccades in distinct populations: east, west, and in-between Knox, Paul C. Wolohan, Felicity D. A. Helmy, Mai S. Exp Brain Res Research Article Express saccades are low latency (80–130 ms), visually guided saccades. While their occurrence is encouraged by the use of gap tasks (the fixation target is extinguished 200 ms prior to the saccade target appearing) and suppressed by the use of overlap tasks (the fixation target remains present when the saccade target appears), there are some healthy, adult participants, “express saccade makers” (ESMs), who persist in generating high proportions (> 30%) of express saccades in overlap conditions. These participants are encountered much more frequently in Chinese participant groups than amongst the Caucasian participants tested to date. What is not known is whether this high number of ESMs is only a feature of Chinese participant groups. More broadly, there are few comparative studies of saccade behaviour across large participant groups drawn from different populations. We, therefore, tested an independent group of 70 healthy adult Egyptian participants, using the same equipment and procedures as employed in the previous studies. Each participant was exposed to two blocks of 200 gap, and two blocks of 200 overlap trials, with block order counterbalanced. Results from the Schwartz Value Survey were used to confirm that this group of participants was culturally distinct from the Chinese and Caucasian (white British) groups tested previously. Fourteen percent (10/70) of this new group were ESMs, and the pattern of latency distribution in these ESMs was identical to that identified in the other participant groups, with a prominent peak in the express latency range in overlap conditions. Overall, we identified three modes in the distribution of saccade latency in overlap conditions, the timing of which (express peak at 110 ms, subsequent peaks at 160 and 210 ms) were strikingly consistent with our previous observations. That these behavioural patterns of saccade latency are observed consistently in large participant groups, drawn from geographically, ethnically, and culturally distinct populations, suggests that they relate to the underlying architecture of the saccade system. ELECTRONIC SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL: The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s00221-017-5094-1) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users. Springer Berlin Heidelberg 2017-09-27 2017 /pmc/articles/PMC5671528/ /pubmed/28956092 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5094-1 Text en © The Author(s) 2017 Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
spellingShingle Research Article
Knox, Paul C.
Wolohan, Felicity D. A.
Helmy, Mai S.
Express saccades in distinct populations: east, west, and in-between
title Express saccades in distinct populations: east, west, and in-between
title_full Express saccades in distinct populations: east, west, and in-between
title_fullStr Express saccades in distinct populations: east, west, and in-between
title_full_unstemmed Express saccades in distinct populations: east, west, and in-between
title_short Express saccades in distinct populations: east, west, and in-between
title_sort express saccades in distinct populations: east, west, and in-between
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5671528/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28956092
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5094-1
work_keys_str_mv AT knoxpaulc expresssaccadesindistinctpopulationseastwestandinbetween
AT wolohanfelicityda expresssaccadesindistinctpopulationseastwestandinbetween
AT helmymais expresssaccadesindistinctpopulationseastwestandinbetween