Cargando…
Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children
School-aged children erroneously think that 1.45 is larger 1.5 because 45 is larger than 5. Using a negative priming paradigm, we investigated whether the ability to compare the magnitude of decimal numbers in the context in which the smallest number has the greatest number of digits after the decim...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Public Library of Science
2017
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5695764/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29155893 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188276 |
_version_ | 1783280354971353088 |
---|---|
author | Roell, Margot Viarouge, Arnaud Houdé, Olivier Borst, Grégoire |
author_facet | Roell, Margot Viarouge, Arnaud Houdé, Olivier Borst, Grégoire |
author_sort | Roell, Margot |
collection | PubMed |
description | School-aged children erroneously think that 1.45 is larger 1.5 because 45 is larger than 5. Using a negative priming paradigm, we investigated whether the ability to compare the magnitude of decimal numbers in the context in which the smallest number has the greatest number of digits after the decimal point (1.45 vs. 1.5) is rooted in part on the ability to inhibit the “greater the number of digits the greater its magnitude” misconception derived from a property of whole numbers. In Experiment 1, we found a typical negative priming effect with 7(th) graders requiring more time to compare decimal numbers in which the largest number has the greatest number of digits after the decimal point (1.65 vs. 1.5) after comparing decimal numbers in which the smallest number has the greatest number of digits after the decimal point (1.45 vs. 1.5) than after comparing decimal numbers with the same number of digits after the decimal point (1.5 vs. 1.6). In Experiment 2, we found a negative priming effect when decimal numbers preceded items in which 7(th) graders had to compare the length of two lines. Taken together our results suggest that the ability to compare decimal numbers in which the smallest number has the greatest number of digits is rooted in part on the ability to inhibit the “greater the number of digits the greater its magnitude” misconception and in part on the ability to inhibit the length of the decimal number per se. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-5695764 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2017 |
publisher | Public Library of Science |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-56957642017-11-30 Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children Roell, Margot Viarouge, Arnaud Houdé, Olivier Borst, Grégoire PLoS One Research Article School-aged children erroneously think that 1.45 is larger 1.5 because 45 is larger than 5. Using a negative priming paradigm, we investigated whether the ability to compare the magnitude of decimal numbers in the context in which the smallest number has the greatest number of digits after the decimal point (1.45 vs. 1.5) is rooted in part on the ability to inhibit the “greater the number of digits the greater its magnitude” misconception derived from a property of whole numbers. In Experiment 1, we found a typical negative priming effect with 7(th) graders requiring more time to compare decimal numbers in which the largest number has the greatest number of digits after the decimal point (1.65 vs. 1.5) after comparing decimal numbers in which the smallest number has the greatest number of digits after the decimal point (1.45 vs. 1.5) than after comparing decimal numbers with the same number of digits after the decimal point (1.5 vs. 1.6). In Experiment 2, we found a negative priming effect when decimal numbers preceded items in which 7(th) graders had to compare the length of two lines. Taken together our results suggest that the ability to compare decimal numbers in which the smallest number has the greatest number of digits is rooted in part on the ability to inhibit the “greater the number of digits the greater its magnitude” misconception and in part on the ability to inhibit the length of the decimal number per se. Public Library of Science 2017-11-20 /pmc/articles/PMC5695764/ /pubmed/29155893 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188276 Text en © 2017 Roell et al http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. |
spellingShingle | Research Article Roell, Margot Viarouge, Arnaud Houdé, Olivier Borst, Grégoire Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children |
title | Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children |
title_full | Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children |
title_fullStr | Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children |
title_full_unstemmed | Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children |
title_short | Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children |
title_sort | inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children |
topic | Research Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5695764/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29155893 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188276 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT roellmargot inhibitorycontrolanddecimalnumbercomparisoninschoolagedchildren AT viarougearnaud inhibitorycontrolanddecimalnumbercomparisoninschoolagedchildren AT houdeolivier inhibitorycontrolanddecimalnumbercomparisoninschoolagedchildren AT borstgregoire inhibitorycontrolanddecimalnumbercomparisoninschoolagedchildren |