Cargando…

Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children

School-aged children erroneously think that 1.45 is larger 1.5 because 45 is larger than 5. Using a negative priming paradigm, we investigated whether the ability to compare the magnitude of decimal numbers in the context in which the smallest number has the greatest number of digits after the decim...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Roell, Margot, Viarouge, Arnaud, Houdé, Olivier, Borst, Grégoire
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Public Library of Science 2017
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5695764/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29155893
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188276
_version_ 1783280354971353088
author Roell, Margot
Viarouge, Arnaud
Houdé, Olivier
Borst, Grégoire
author_facet Roell, Margot
Viarouge, Arnaud
Houdé, Olivier
Borst, Grégoire
author_sort Roell, Margot
collection PubMed
description School-aged children erroneously think that 1.45 is larger 1.5 because 45 is larger than 5. Using a negative priming paradigm, we investigated whether the ability to compare the magnitude of decimal numbers in the context in which the smallest number has the greatest number of digits after the decimal point (1.45 vs. 1.5) is rooted in part on the ability to inhibit the “greater the number of digits the greater its magnitude” misconception derived from a property of whole numbers. In Experiment 1, we found a typical negative priming effect with 7(th) graders requiring more time to compare decimal numbers in which the largest number has the greatest number of digits after the decimal point (1.65 vs. 1.5) after comparing decimal numbers in which the smallest number has the greatest number of digits after the decimal point (1.45 vs. 1.5) than after comparing decimal numbers with the same number of digits after the decimal point (1.5 vs. 1.6). In Experiment 2, we found a negative priming effect when decimal numbers preceded items in which 7(th) graders had to compare the length of two lines. Taken together our results suggest that the ability to compare decimal numbers in which the smallest number has the greatest number of digits is rooted in part on the ability to inhibit the “greater the number of digits the greater its magnitude” misconception and in part on the ability to inhibit the length of the decimal number per se.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-5695764
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2017
publisher Public Library of Science
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-56957642017-11-30 Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children Roell, Margot Viarouge, Arnaud Houdé, Olivier Borst, Grégoire PLoS One Research Article School-aged children erroneously think that 1.45 is larger 1.5 because 45 is larger than 5. Using a negative priming paradigm, we investigated whether the ability to compare the magnitude of decimal numbers in the context in which the smallest number has the greatest number of digits after the decimal point (1.45 vs. 1.5) is rooted in part on the ability to inhibit the “greater the number of digits the greater its magnitude” misconception derived from a property of whole numbers. In Experiment 1, we found a typical negative priming effect with 7(th) graders requiring more time to compare decimal numbers in which the largest number has the greatest number of digits after the decimal point (1.65 vs. 1.5) after comparing decimal numbers in which the smallest number has the greatest number of digits after the decimal point (1.45 vs. 1.5) than after comparing decimal numbers with the same number of digits after the decimal point (1.5 vs. 1.6). In Experiment 2, we found a negative priming effect when decimal numbers preceded items in which 7(th) graders had to compare the length of two lines. Taken together our results suggest that the ability to compare decimal numbers in which the smallest number has the greatest number of digits is rooted in part on the ability to inhibit the “greater the number of digits the greater its magnitude” misconception and in part on the ability to inhibit the length of the decimal number per se. Public Library of Science 2017-11-20 /pmc/articles/PMC5695764/ /pubmed/29155893 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188276 Text en © 2017 Roell et al http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
spellingShingle Research Article
Roell, Margot
Viarouge, Arnaud
Houdé, Olivier
Borst, Grégoire
Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children
title Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children
title_full Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children
title_fullStr Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children
title_full_unstemmed Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children
title_short Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children
title_sort inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5695764/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29155893
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188276
work_keys_str_mv AT roellmargot inhibitorycontrolanddecimalnumbercomparisoninschoolagedchildren
AT viarougearnaud inhibitorycontrolanddecimalnumbercomparisoninschoolagedchildren
AT houdeolivier inhibitorycontrolanddecimalnumbercomparisoninschoolagedchildren
AT borstgregoire inhibitorycontrolanddecimalnumbercomparisoninschoolagedchildren