Cargando…
Beating time: How ensemble musicians’ cueing gestures communicate beat position and tempo
Ensemble musicians typically exchange visual cues to coordinate piece entrances. “Cueing-in” gestures indicate when to begin playing and at what tempo. This study investigated how timing information is encoded in musicians’ cueing-in gestures. Gesture acceleration patterns were expected to indicate...
Autores principales: | , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
SAGE Publications
2017
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5718341/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29276332 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0305735617702971 |
_version_ | 1783284308707901440 |
---|---|
author | Bishop, Laura Goebl, Werner |
author_facet | Bishop, Laura Goebl, Werner |
author_sort | Bishop, Laura |
collection | PubMed |
description | Ensemble musicians typically exchange visual cues to coordinate piece entrances. “Cueing-in” gestures indicate when to begin playing and at what tempo. This study investigated how timing information is encoded in musicians’ cueing-in gestures. Gesture acceleration patterns were expected to indicate beat position, while gesture periodicity, duration, and peak gesture velocity were expected to indicate tempo. Same-instrument ensembles (e.g., piano–piano) were expected to synchronize more successfully than mixed-instrument ensembles (e.g., piano–violin). Duos performed short passages as their head and (for violinists) bowing hand movements were tracked with accelerometers and Kinect sensors. Performers alternated between leader/follower roles; leaders heard a tempo via headphones and cued their partner in nonverbally. Violin duos synchronized more successfully than either piano duos or piano–violin duos, possibly because violinists were more experienced in ensemble playing than pianists. Peak acceleration indicated beat position in leaders’ head-nodding gestures. Gesture duration and periodicity in leaders’ head and bowing hand gestures indicated tempo. The results show that the spatio-temporal characteristics of cueing-in gestures guide beat perception, enabling synchronization with visual gestures that follow a range of spatial trajectories. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-5718341 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2017 |
publisher | SAGE Publications |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-57183412017-12-20 Beating time: How ensemble musicians’ cueing gestures communicate beat position and tempo Bishop, Laura Goebl, Werner Psychol Music Articles Ensemble musicians typically exchange visual cues to coordinate piece entrances. “Cueing-in” gestures indicate when to begin playing and at what tempo. This study investigated how timing information is encoded in musicians’ cueing-in gestures. Gesture acceleration patterns were expected to indicate beat position, while gesture periodicity, duration, and peak gesture velocity were expected to indicate tempo. Same-instrument ensembles (e.g., piano–piano) were expected to synchronize more successfully than mixed-instrument ensembles (e.g., piano–violin). Duos performed short passages as their head and (for violinists) bowing hand movements were tracked with accelerometers and Kinect sensors. Performers alternated between leader/follower roles; leaders heard a tempo via headphones and cued their partner in nonverbally. Violin duos synchronized more successfully than either piano duos or piano–violin duos, possibly because violinists were more experienced in ensemble playing than pianists. Peak acceleration indicated beat position in leaders’ head-nodding gestures. Gesture duration and periodicity in leaders’ head and bowing hand gestures indicated tempo. The results show that the spatio-temporal characteristics of cueing-in gestures guide beat perception, enabling synchronization with visual gestures that follow a range of spatial trajectories. SAGE Publications 2017-04-27 2018-01 /pmc/articles/PMC5718341/ /pubmed/29276332 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0305735617702971 Text en © The Author(s) 2017 http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/ This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access page (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage). |
spellingShingle | Articles Bishop, Laura Goebl, Werner Beating time: How ensemble musicians’ cueing gestures communicate beat position and tempo |
title | Beating time: How ensemble musicians’ cueing gestures communicate beat position and tempo |
title_full | Beating time: How ensemble musicians’ cueing gestures communicate beat position and tempo |
title_fullStr | Beating time: How ensemble musicians’ cueing gestures communicate beat position and tempo |
title_full_unstemmed | Beating time: How ensemble musicians’ cueing gestures communicate beat position and tempo |
title_short | Beating time: How ensemble musicians’ cueing gestures communicate beat position and tempo |
title_sort | beating time: how ensemble musicians’ cueing gestures communicate beat position and tempo |
topic | Articles |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5718341/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29276332 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0305735617702971 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT bishoplaura beatingtimehowensemblemusicianscueinggesturescommunicatebeatpositionandtempo AT goeblwerner beatingtimehowensemblemusicianscueinggesturescommunicatebeatpositionandtempo |