Cargando…
Data-Gathering, Belief Flexibility, and Reasoning Across the Psychosis Continuum
BACKGROUND: There is evidence for a group of nonclinical individuals with full-blown, persistent psychotic experiences (PEs) but no need-for-care: they are of particular importance in identifying risk and protective factors for clinical psychosis. The aim of this study was to investigate whether rea...
Autores principales: | , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Oxford University Press
2018
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5768047/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28338872 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx029 |
_version_ | 1783292639823527936 |
---|---|
author | Ward, Thomas Peters, Emmanuelle Jackson, Mike Day, Fern Garety, Philippa A |
author_facet | Ward, Thomas Peters, Emmanuelle Jackson, Mike Day, Fern Garety, Philippa A |
author_sort | Ward, Thomas |
collection | PubMed |
description | BACKGROUND: There is evidence for a group of nonclinical individuals with full-blown, persistent psychotic experiences (PEs) but no need-for-care: they are of particular importance in identifying risk and protective factors for clinical psychosis. The aim of this study was to investigate whether reasoning biases are related to PEs or need-for-care. METHOD: Two groups with persistent PEs (clinical; n = 74; nonclinical; n = 92) and a control group without PEs (n = 83) were compared on jumping-to-conclusions (JTC) and belief flexibility. A randomly selected subset of interviews (n = 104) was analyzed to examine differences in experiential and rational reasoning. RESULTS: As predicted JTC was more common in the clinical than the other 2 groups. Unexpectedly no group differences were observed between clinical and nonclinical groups on measures of belief flexibility. However, the clinical group was less likely to employ rational reasoning, while the nonclinical group was more likely to use experiential reasoning plus a combination of both types of reasoning processes, compared to the other 2 groups. CONCLUSIONS: Reasoning biases differ in groups with PEs with and without need-for-care. JTC is associated with need-for-care rather than with PEs. The ability to invoke rational reasoning processes, together with an absence of JTC, may protect against pathological outcomes of persistent PEs. However, marked use of experiential reasoning is associated with the occurrence of PEs in both clinical and nonclinical groups. Implications for theory development, intervention and further research are discussed. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-5768047 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2018 |
publisher | Oxford University Press |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-57680472018-01-19 Data-Gathering, Belief Flexibility, and Reasoning Across the Psychosis Continuum Ward, Thomas Peters, Emmanuelle Jackson, Mike Day, Fern Garety, Philippa A Schizophr Bull Regular Articles BACKGROUND: There is evidence for a group of nonclinical individuals with full-blown, persistent psychotic experiences (PEs) but no need-for-care: they are of particular importance in identifying risk and protective factors for clinical psychosis. The aim of this study was to investigate whether reasoning biases are related to PEs or need-for-care. METHOD: Two groups with persistent PEs (clinical; n = 74; nonclinical; n = 92) and a control group without PEs (n = 83) were compared on jumping-to-conclusions (JTC) and belief flexibility. A randomly selected subset of interviews (n = 104) was analyzed to examine differences in experiential and rational reasoning. RESULTS: As predicted JTC was more common in the clinical than the other 2 groups. Unexpectedly no group differences were observed between clinical and nonclinical groups on measures of belief flexibility. However, the clinical group was less likely to employ rational reasoning, while the nonclinical group was more likely to use experiential reasoning plus a combination of both types of reasoning processes, compared to the other 2 groups. CONCLUSIONS: Reasoning biases differ in groups with PEs with and without need-for-care. JTC is associated with need-for-care rather than with PEs. The ability to invoke rational reasoning processes, together with an absence of JTC, may protect against pathological outcomes of persistent PEs. However, marked use of experiential reasoning is associated with the occurrence of PEs in both clinical and nonclinical groups. Implications for theory development, intervention and further research are discussed. Oxford University Press 2018-01 2017-03-08 /pmc/articles/PMC5768047/ /pubmed/28338872 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx029 Text en © The Author(s) 2017. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Maryland Psychiatric Research Center. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. |
spellingShingle | Regular Articles Ward, Thomas Peters, Emmanuelle Jackson, Mike Day, Fern Garety, Philippa A Data-Gathering, Belief Flexibility, and Reasoning Across the Psychosis Continuum |
title | Data-Gathering, Belief Flexibility, and Reasoning Across the Psychosis Continuum |
title_full | Data-Gathering, Belief Flexibility, and Reasoning Across the Psychosis Continuum |
title_fullStr | Data-Gathering, Belief Flexibility, and Reasoning Across the Psychosis Continuum |
title_full_unstemmed | Data-Gathering, Belief Flexibility, and Reasoning Across the Psychosis Continuum |
title_short | Data-Gathering, Belief Flexibility, and Reasoning Across the Psychosis Continuum |
title_sort | data-gathering, belief flexibility, and reasoning across the psychosis continuum |
topic | Regular Articles |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5768047/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28338872 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx029 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT wardthomas datagatheringbeliefflexibilityandreasoningacrossthepsychosiscontinuum AT petersemmanuelle datagatheringbeliefflexibilityandreasoningacrossthepsychosiscontinuum AT jacksonmike datagatheringbeliefflexibilityandreasoningacrossthepsychosiscontinuum AT dayfern datagatheringbeliefflexibilityandreasoningacrossthepsychosiscontinuum AT garetyphilippaa datagatheringbeliefflexibilityandreasoningacrossthepsychosiscontinuum |