Cargando…

Social Context Disambiguates the Interpretation of Laughter

Despite being a pan-cultural phenomenon, laughter is arguably the least understood behaviour deployed in social interaction. As well as being a response to humour, it has other important functions including promoting social affiliation, developing cooperation and regulating competitive behaviours. T...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Curran, William, McKeown, Gary J., Rychlowska, Magdalena, André, Elisabeth, Wagner, Johannes, Lingenfelser, Florian
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Frontiers Media S.A. 2018
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5770603/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29375448
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02342
_version_ 1783293102462599168
author Curran, William
McKeown, Gary J.
Rychlowska, Magdalena
André, Elisabeth
Wagner, Johannes
Lingenfelser, Florian
author_facet Curran, William
McKeown, Gary J.
Rychlowska, Magdalena
André, Elisabeth
Wagner, Johannes
Lingenfelser, Florian
author_sort Curran, William
collection PubMed
description Despite being a pan-cultural phenomenon, laughter is arguably the least understood behaviour deployed in social interaction. As well as being a response to humour, it has other important functions including promoting social affiliation, developing cooperation and regulating competitive behaviours. This multi-functional feature of laughter marks it as an adaptive behaviour central to facilitating social cohesion. However, it is not clear how laughter achieves this social cohesion. We consider two approaches to understanding how laughter facilitates social cohesion – the ‘representational’ approach and the ‘affect-induction’ approach. The representational approach suggests that laughter conveys information about the expresser’s emotional state, and the listener decodes this information to gain knowledge about the laugher’s felt state. The affect-induction approach views laughter as a tool to influence the affective state of listeners. We describe a modified version of the affect-induction approach, in which laughter is combined with additional factors – including social context, verbal information, other social signals and knowledge of the listener’s emotional state – to influence an interaction partner. This view asserts that laughter by itself is ambiguous: the same laughter may induce positive or negative affect in a listener, with the outcome determined by the combination of these additional factors. Here we describe two experiments exploring which of these approaches accurately describes laughter. Participants judged the genuineness of audio–video recordings of social interactions containing laughter. Unknown to the participants the recordings contained either the original laughter or replacement laughter from a different part of the interaction. When replacement laughter was matched for intensity, genuineness judgements were similar to judgements of the original unmodified recordings. When replacement laughter was not matched for intensity, genuineness judgements were generally significantly lower. These results support the affect-induction view of laughter by suggesting that laughter is inherently underdetermined and ambiguous, and that its interpretation is determined by the context in which it occurs.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-5770603
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2018
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-57706032018-01-26 Social Context Disambiguates the Interpretation of Laughter Curran, William McKeown, Gary J. Rychlowska, Magdalena André, Elisabeth Wagner, Johannes Lingenfelser, Florian Front Psychol Psychology Despite being a pan-cultural phenomenon, laughter is arguably the least understood behaviour deployed in social interaction. As well as being a response to humour, it has other important functions including promoting social affiliation, developing cooperation and regulating competitive behaviours. This multi-functional feature of laughter marks it as an adaptive behaviour central to facilitating social cohesion. However, it is not clear how laughter achieves this social cohesion. We consider two approaches to understanding how laughter facilitates social cohesion – the ‘representational’ approach and the ‘affect-induction’ approach. The representational approach suggests that laughter conveys information about the expresser’s emotional state, and the listener decodes this information to gain knowledge about the laugher’s felt state. The affect-induction approach views laughter as a tool to influence the affective state of listeners. We describe a modified version of the affect-induction approach, in which laughter is combined with additional factors – including social context, verbal information, other social signals and knowledge of the listener’s emotional state – to influence an interaction partner. This view asserts that laughter by itself is ambiguous: the same laughter may induce positive or negative affect in a listener, with the outcome determined by the combination of these additional factors. Here we describe two experiments exploring which of these approaches accurately describes laughter. Participants judged the genuineness of audio–video recordings of social interactions containing laughter. Unknown to the participants the recordings contained either the original laughter or replacement laughter from a different part of the interaction. When replacement laughter was matched for intensity, genuineness judgements were similar to judgements of the original unmodified recordings. When replacement laughter was not matched for intensity, genuineness judgements were generally significantly lower. These results support the affect-induction view of laughter by suggesting that laughter is inherently underdetermined and ambiguous, and that its interpretation is determined by the context in which it occurs. Frontiers Media S.A. 2018-01-12 /pmc/articles/PMC5770603/ /pubmed/29375448 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02342 Text en Copyright © 2018 Curran, McKeown, Rychlowska, André, Wagner and Lingenfelser. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
spellingShingle Psychology
Curran, William
McKeown, Gary J.
Rychlowska, Magdalena
André, Elisabeth
Wagner, Johannes
Lingenfelser, Florian
Social Context Disambiguates the Interpretation of Laughter
title Social Context Disambiguates the Interpretation of Laughter
title_full Social Context Disambiguates the Interpretation of Laughter
title_fullStr Social Context Disambiguates the Interpretation of Laughter
title_full_unstemmed Social Context Disambiguates the Interpretation of Laughter
title_short Social Context Disambiguates the Interpretation of Laughter
title_sort social context disambiguates the interpretation of laughter
topic Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5770603/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29375448
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02342
work_keys_str_mv AT curranwilliam socialcontextdisambiguatestheinterpretationoflaughter
AT mckeowngaryj socialcontextdisambiguatestheinterpretationoflaughter
AT rychlowskamagdalena socialcontextdisambiguatestheinterpretationoflaughter
AT andreelisabeth socialcontextdisambiguatestheinterpretationoflaughter
AT wagnerjohannes socialcontextdisambiguatestheinterpretationoflaughter
AT lingenfelserflorian socialcontextdisambiguatestheinterpretationoflaughter