Cargando…

Effects of Language Background on Gaze Behavior: A Crosslinguistic Comparison Between Korean and German Speakers

Languages differ in how they categorize spatial relations: While German differentiates between containment (in) and support (auf) with distinct spatial words—(a) den Kuli IN die Kappe stecken (”put pen in cap”); (b) die Kappe AUF den Kuli stecken (”put cap on pen”)—Korean uses a single spatial word...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Goller, Florian, Lee, Donghoon, Ansorge, Ulrich, Choi, Soonja
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: University of Finance and Management in Warsaw 2017
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5770776/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29362644
http://dx.doi.org/10.5709/acp-0227-z
_version_ 1783293130733256704
author Goller, Florian
Lee, Donghoon
Ansorge, Ulrich
Choi, Soonja
author_facet Goller, Florian
Lee, Donghoon
Ansorge, Ulrich
Choi, Soonja
author_sort Goller, Florian
collection PubMed
description Languages differ in how they categorize spatial relations: While German differentiates between containment (in) and support (auf) with distinct spatial words—(a) den Kuli IN die Kappe stecken (”put pen in cap”); (b) die Kappe AUF den Kuli stecken (”put cap on pen”)—Korean uses a single spatial word (kkita) collapsing (a) and (b) into one semantic category, particularly when the spatial enclosure is tight-fit. Korean uses a different word (i.e., netha) for loose-fits (e.g., apple in bowl). We tested whether these differences influence the attention of the speaker. In a crosslinguistic study, we compared native German speakers with native Korean speakers. Participants rated the similarity of two successive video clips of several scenes where two objects were joined or nested (either in a tight or loose manner). The rating data show that Korean speakers base their rating of similarity more on tight- versus loose-fit, whereas German speakers base their rating more on containment versus support (in vs. auf). Throughout the experiment, we also measured the participants’ eye movements. Korean speakers looked equally long at the moving Figure object and at the stationary Ground object, whereas German speakers were more biased to look at the Ground object. Additionally, Korean speakers also looked more at the region where the two objects touched than did German speakers. We discuss our data in the light of crosslinguistic semantics and the extent of their influence on spatial cognition and perception.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-5770776
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2017
publisher University of Finance and Management in Warsaw
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-57707762018-01-23 Effects of Language Background on Gaze Behavior: A Crosslinguistic Comparison Between Korean and German Speakers Goller, Florian Lee, Donghoon Ansorge, Ulrich Choi, Soonja Adv Cogn Psychol Research Article Languages differ in how they categorize spatial relations: While German differentiates between containment (in) and support (auf) with distinct spatial words—(a) den Kuli IN die Kappe stecken (”put pen in cap”); (b) die Kappe AUF den Kuli stecken (”put cap on pen”)—Korean uses a single spatial word (kkita) collapsing (a) and (b) into one semantic category, particularly when the spatial enclosure is tight-fit. Korean uses a different word (i.e., netha) for loose-fits (e.g., apple in bowl). We tested whether these differences influence the attention of the speaker. In a crosslinguistic study, we compared native German speakers with native Korean speakers. Participants rated the similarity of two successive video clips of several scenes where two objects were joined or nested (either in a tight or loose manner). The rating data show that Korean speakers base their rating of similarity more on tight- versus loose-fit, whereas German speakers base their rating more on containment versus support (in vs. auf). Throughout the experiment, we also measured the participants’ eye movements. Korean speakers looked equally long at the moving Figure object and at the stationary Ground object, whereas German speakers were more biased to look at the Ground object. Additionally, Korean speakers also looked more at the region where the two objects touched than did German speakers. We discuss our data in the light of crosslinguistic semantics and the extent of their influence on spatial cognition and perception. University of Finance and Management in Warsaw 2017-12-31 /pmc/articles/PMC5770776/ /pubmed/29362644 http://dx.doi.org/10.5709/acp-0227-z Text en Copyright: © 2017 The Authors http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
spellingShingle Research Article
Goller, Florian
Lee, Donghoon
Ansorge, Ulrich
Choi, Soonja
Effects of Language Background on Gaze Behavior: A Crosslinguistic Comparison Between Korean and German Speakers
title Effects of Language Background on Gaze Behavior: A Crosslinguistic Comparison Between Korean and German Speakers
title_full Effects of Language Background on Gaze Behavior: A Crosslinguistic Comparison Between Korean and German Speakers
title_fullStr Effects of Language Background on Gaze Behavior: A Crosslinguistic Comparison Between Korean and German Speakers
title_full_unstemmed Effects of Language Background on Gaze Behavior: A Crosslinguistic Comparison Between Korean and German Speakers
title_short Effects of Language Background on Gaze Behavior: A Crosslinguistic Comparison Between Korean and German Speakers
title_sort effects of language background on gaze behavior: a crosslinguistic comparison between korean and german speakers
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5770776/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29362644
http://dx.doi.org/10.5709/acp-0227-z
work_keys_str_mv AT gollerflorian effectsoflanguagebackgroundongazebehavioracrosslinguisticcomparisonbetweenkoreanandgermanspeakers
AT leedonghoon effectsoflanguagebackgroundongazebehavioracrosslinguisticcomparisonbetweenkoreanandgermanspeakers
AT ansorgeulrich effectsoflanguagebackgroundongazebehavioracrosslinguisticcomparisonbetweenkoreanandgermanspeakers
AT choisoonja effectsoflanguagebackgroundongazebehavioracrosslinguisticcomparisonbetweenkoreanandgermanspeakers