Cargando…
Prenatal and perinatal risks for late language emergence in a population-level sample of twins at age 2
BACKGROUND: Late Language Emergence (LLE) in the first two years of life is one of the most common parental concerns about child development and reasons for seeking advice from health professionals. LLE is much more prevalent in twins (38%) than singletons (20%). In studies of language development i...
Autores principales: | , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
BioMed Central
2018
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5804054/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29415681 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12887-018-1035-9 |
_version_ | 1783298761474179072 |
---|---|
author | Taylor, Catherine L. Rice, Mabel L. Christensen, Daniel Blair, Eve Zubrick, Stephen R. |
author_facet | Taylor, Catherine L. Rice, Mabel L. Christensen, Daniel Blair, Eve Zubrick, Stephen R. |
author_sort | Taylor, Catherine L. |
collection | PubMed |
description | BACKGROUND: Late Language Emergence (LLE) in the first two years of life is one of the most common parental concerns about child development and reasons for seeking advice from health professionals. LLE is much more prevalent in twins (38%) than singletons (20%). In studies of language development in twins without overt disability, adverse prenatal and perinatal environments have been reported to play a lesser role in the etiology of LLE than adverse postnatal environments. However, there is a lack of population-level evidence about prenatal and perinatal risk factors for LLE in twins. This study investigated the extent to which prenatal and perinatal risk factors were associated with LLE in a population-level sample of twins at age 2 without overt disability. METHODS: The sample comprised 473 twin pairs drawn from a population sample frame comprising statutory notifications of all births in Western Australia (WA), 2000–2003. Twin pairs in which either twin had a known developmental disorder or exposure to language(s) other than English were excluded. Of the 946 twins, 47.9% were male. There were 313 dizygotic and 160 monozygotic twin pairs. LLE was defined as a score at or below the gender-specific 10th percentile on the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: Words and Sentences (CDI-WS) (Words Produced). Bivariate and multivariable logistic regression was used to investigate risk factors associated with LLE. RESULTS: In the multivariable model, risk factors for LLE in order of decreasing magnitude were: Gestational diabetes had an adjusted odds ratio (aOR) of 19.5 (95% confidence interval (CI) 1.2, 313.1); prolonged TSR (aOR: 13.6 [2.0, 91.1]); multiparity (aOR: 7.6 [1.6, 37.5]), monozygosity (aOR: 6.9 [1.7, 27.9]) and fetal growth restriction (aOR: 4.6 [1.7, 12.7]). Sociodemographic risk factors (e.g., low maternal education, socioeconomic area disadvantage) were not associated with increased odds of LLE. CONCLUSIONS: The results suggest that adverse prenatal and perinatal environments are important in the etiology of LLE in twins at age 2. It is important that health professionals discuss twin pregnancy and birth risks for delayed speech and language milestones with parents and provide ongoing developmental monitoring for all twins, not just twins with overt disability. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-5804054 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2018 |
publisher | BioMed Central |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-58040542018-02-14 Prenatal and perinatal risks for late language emergence in a population-level sample of twins at age 2 Taylor, Catherine L. Rice, Mabel L. Christensen, Daniel Blair, Eve Zubrick, Stephen R. BMC Pediatr Research Article BACKGROUND: Late Language Emergence (LLE) in the first two years of life is one of the most common parental concerns about child development and reasons for seeking advice from health professionals. LLE is much more prevalent in twins (38%) than singletons (20%). In studies of language development in twins without overt disability, adverse prenatal and perinatal environments have been reported to play a lesser role in the etiology of LLE than adverse postnatal environments. However, there is a lack of population-level evidence about prenatal and perinatal risk factors for LLE in twins. This study investigated the extent to which prenatal and perinatal risk factors were associated with LLE in a population-level sample of twins at age 2 without overt disability. METHODS: The sample comprised 473 twin pairs drawn from a population sample frame comprising statutory notifications of all births in Western Australia (WA), 2000–2003. Twin pairs in which either twin had a known developmental disorder or exposure to language(s) other than English were excluded. Of the 946 twins, 47.9% were male. There were 313 dizygotic and 160 monozygotic twin pairs. LLE was defined as a score at or below the gender-specific 10th percentile on the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: Words and Sentences (CDI-WS) (Words Produced). Bivariate and multivariable logistic regression was used to investigate risk factors associated with LLE. RESULTS: In the multivariable model, risk factors for LLE in order of decreasing magnitude were: Gestational diabetes had an adjusted odds ratio (aOR) of 19.5 (95% confidence interval (CI) 1.2, 313.1); prolonged TSR (aOR: 13.6 [2.0, 91.1]); multiparity (aOR: 7.6 [1.6, 37.5]), monozygosity (aOR: 6.9 [1.7, 27.9]) and fetal growth restriction (aOR: 4.6 [1.7, 12.7]). Sociodemographic risk factors (e.g., low maternal education, socioeconomic area disadvantage) were not associated with increased odds of LLE. CONCLUSIONS: The results suggest that adverse prenatal and perinatal environments are important in the etiology of LLE in twins at age 2. It is important that health professionals discuss twin pregnancy and birth risks for delayed speech and language milestones with parents and provide ongoing developmental monitoring for all twins, not just twins with overt disability. BioMed Central 2018-02-07 /pmc/articles/PMC5804054/ /pubmed/29415681 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12887-018-1035-9 Text en © The Author(s). 2018 Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated. |
spellingShingle | Research Article Taylor, Catherine L. Rice, Mabel L. Christensen, Daniel Blair, Eve Zubrick, Stephen R. Prenatal and perinatal risks for late language emergence in a population-level sample of twins at age 2 |
title | Prenatal and perinatal risks for late language emergence in a population-level sample of twins at age 2 |
title_full | Prenatal and perinatal risks for late language emergence in a population-level sample of twins at age 2 |
title_fullStr | Prenatal and perinatal risks for late language emergence in a population-level sample of twins at age 2 |
title_full_unstemmed | Prenatal and perinatal risks for late language emergence in a population-level sample of twins at age 2 |
title_short | Prenatal and perinatal risks for late language emergence in a population-level sample of twins at age 2 |
title_sort | prenatal and perinatal risks for late language emergence in a population-level sample of twins at age 2 |
topic | Research Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5804054/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29415681 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12887-018-1035-9 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT taylorcatherinel prenatalandperinatalrisksforlatelanguageemergenceinapopulationlevelsampleoftwinsatage2 AT ricemabell prenatalandperinatalrisksforlatelanguageemergenceinapopulationlevelsampleoftwinsatage2 AT christensendaniel prenatalandperinatalrisksforlatelanguageemergenceinapopulationlevelsampleoftwinsatage2 AT blaireve prenatalandperinatalrisksforlatelanguageemergenceinapopulationlevelsampleoftwinsatage2 AT zubrickstephenr prenatalandperinatalrisksforlatelanguageemergenceinapopulationlevelsampleoftwinsatage2 |