Cargando…
Visual Features in the Perception of Liquids
Perceptual constancy—identifying surfaces and objects across large image changes—remains an important challenge for visual neuroscience [1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8]. Liquids are particularly challenging because they respond to external forces in complex, highly variable ways, presenting an enormous rang...
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Cell Press
2018
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5807092/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29395924 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.037 |
_version_ | 1783299227580891136 |
---|---|
author | van Assen, Jan Jaap R. Barla, Pascal Fleming, Roland W. |
author_facet | van Assen, Jan Jaap R. Barla, Pascal Fleming, Roland W. |
author_sort | van Assen, Jan Jaap R. |
collection | PubMed |
description | Perceptual constancy—identifying surfaces and objects across large image changes—remains an important challenge for visual neuroscience [1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8]. Liquids are particularly challenging because they respond to external forces in complex, highly variable ways, presenting an enormous range of images to the visual system. To achieve constancy, the brain must perform a causal inference [9, 10, 11] that disentangles the liquid’s viscosity from external factors—like gravity and object interactions—that also affect the liquid’s behavior. Here, we tested whether the visual system estimates viscosity using “midlevel” features [12, 13, 14] that respond more to viscosity than other factors. Observers reported the perceived viscosity of simulated liquids ranging from water to molten glass exhibiting diverse behaviors (e.g., pouring, stirring). A separate group of observers rated the same animations for 20 midlevel 3D shape and motion features. Applying factor analysis to the feature ratings reveals that a weighted combination of four underlying factors (distribution, irregularity, rectilinearity, and dynamics) predicted perceived viscosity very well across this wide range of contexts (R(2) = 0.93). Interestingly, observers unknowingly ordered their midlevel judgments according to the one common factor across contexts: variation in viscosity. Principal component analysis reveals that across the features, the first component lines up almost perfectly with the viscosity (R(2) = 0.96). Our findings demonstrate that the visual system achieves constancy by representing stimuli in a multidimensional feature space—based on complementary, midlevel features—which successfully cluster very different stimuli together and tease similar stimuli apart, so that viscosity can be read out easily. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-5807092 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2018 |
publisher | Cell Press |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-58070922018-02-13 Visual Features in the Perception of Liquids van Assen, Jan Jaap R. Barla, Pascal Fleming, Roland W. Curr Biol Article Perceptual constancy—identifying surfaces and objects across large image changes—remains an important challenge for visual neuroscience [1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8]. Liquids are particularly challenging because they respond to external forces in complex, highly variable ways, presenting an enormous range of images to the visual system. To achieve constancy, the brain must perform a causal inference [9, 10, 11] that disentangles the liquid’s viscosity from external factors—like gravity and object interactions—that also affect the liquid’s behavior. Here, we tested whether the visual system estimates viscosity using “midlevel” features [12, 13, 14] that respond more to viscosity than other factors. Observers reported the perceived viscosity of simulated liquids ranging from water to molten glass exhibiting diverse behaviors (e.g., pouring, stirring). A separate group of observers rated the same animations for 20 midlevel 3D shape and motion features. Applying factor analysis to the feature ratings reveals that a weighted combination of four underlying factors (distribution, irregularity, rectilinearity, and dynamics) predicted perceived viscosity very well across this wide range of contexts (R(2) = 0.93). Interestingly, observers unknowingly ordered their midlevel judgments according to the one common factor across contexts: variation in viscosity. Principal component analysis reveals that across the features, the first component lines up almost perfectly with the viscosity (R(2) = 0.96). Our findings demonstrate that the visual system achieves constancy by representing stimuli in a multidimensional feature space—based on complementary, midlevel features—which successfully cluster very different stimuli together and tease similar stimuli apart, so that viscosity can be read out easily. Cell Press 2018-02-05 /pmc/articles/PMC5807092/ /pubmed/29395924 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.037 Text en © 2018 The Authors http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). |
spellingShingle | Article van Assen, Jan Jaap R. Barla, Pascal Fleming, Roland W. Visual Features in the Perception of Liquids |
title | Visual Features in the Perception of Liquids |
title_full | Visual Features in the Perception of Liquids |
title_fullStr | Visual Features in the Perception of Liquids |
title_full_unstemmed | Visual Features in the Perception of Liquids |
title_short | Visual Features in the Perception of Liquids |
title_sort | visual features in the perception of liquids |
topic | Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5807092/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29395924 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.037 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT vanassenjanjaapr visualfeaturesintheperceptionofliquids AT barlapascal visualfeaturesintheperceptionofliquids AT flemingrolandw visualfeaturesintheperceptionofliquids |