Cargando…
Self domestication and the evolution of language
We set out an account of how self-domestication plays a crucial role in the evolution of language. In doing so, we focus on the growing body of work that treats language structure as emerging from the process of cultural transmission. We argue that a full recognition of the importance of cultural tr...
Autores principales: | , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Springer Netherlands
2018
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5871649/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29606782 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10539-018-9612-8 |
_version_ | 1783309668361175040 |
---|---|
author | Thomas, James Kirby, Simon |
author_facet | Thomas, James Kirby, Simon |
author_sort | Thomas, James |
collection | PubMed |
description | We set out an account of how self-domestication plays a crucial role in the evolution of language. In doing so, we focus on the growing body of work that treats language structure as emerging from the process of cultural transmission. We argue that a full recognition of the importance of cultural transmission fundamentally changes the kind of questions we should be asking regarding the biological basis of language structure. If we think of language structure as reflecting an accumulated set of changes in our genome, then we might ask something like, “What are the genetic bases of language structure and why were they selected?” However, if cultural evolution can account for language structure, then this question no longer applies. Instead, we face the task of accounting for the origin of the traits that enabled that process of structure-creating cultural evolution to get started in the first place. In light of work on cultural evolution, then, the new question for biological evolution becomes, “How did those precursor traits evolve?” We identify two key precursor traits: (1) the transmission of the communication system through learning; and (2) the ability to infer the communicative intent associated with a signal or action. We then describe two comparative case studies—the Bengalese finch and the domestic dog—in which parallel traits can be seen emerging following domestication. Finally, we turn to the role of domestication in human evolution. We argue that the cultural evolution of language structure has its origin in an earlier process of self-domestication. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-5871649 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2018 |
publisher | Springer Netherlands |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-58716492018-03-28 Self domestication and the evolution of language Thomas, James Kirby, Simon Biol Philos Article We set out an account of how self-domestication plays a crucial role in the evolution of language. In doing so, we focus on the growing body of work that treats language structure as emerging from the process of cultural transmission. We argue that a full recognition of the importance of cultural transmission fundamentally changes the kind of questions we should be asking regarding the biological basis of language structure. If we think of language structure as reflecting an accumulated set of changes in our genome, then we might ask something like, “What are the genetic bases of language structure and why were they selected?” However, if cultural evolution can account for language structure, then this question no longer applies. Instead, we face the task of accounting for the origin of the traits that enabled that process of structure-creating cultural evolution to get started in the first place. In light of work on cultural evolution, then, the new question for biological evolution becomes, “How did those precursor traits evolve?” We identify two key precursor traits: (1) the transmission of the communication system through learning; and (2) the ability to infer the communicative intent associated with a signal or action. We then describe two comparative case studies—the Bengalese finch and the domestic dog—in which parallel traits can be seen emerging following domestication. Finally, we turn to the role of domestication in human evolution. We argue that the cultural evolution of language structure has its origin in an earlier process of self-domestication. Springer Netherlands 2018-03-27 2018 /pmc/articles/PMC5871649/ /pubmed/29606782 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10539-018-9612-8 Text en © The Author(s) 2018 Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. |
spellingShingle | Article Thomas, James Kirby, Simon Self domestication and the evolution of language |
title | Self domestication and the evolution of language |
title_full | Self domestication and the evolution of language |
title_fullStr | Self domestication and the evolution of language |
title_full_unstemmed | Self domestication and the evolution of language |
title_short | Self domestication and the evolution of language |
title_sort | self domestication and the evolution of language |
topic | Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5871649/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29606782 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10539-018-9612-8 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT thomasjames selfdomesticationandtheevolutionoflanguage AT kirbysimon selfdomesticationandtheevolutionoflanguage |