Cargando…
Backlash against gender stereotype-violating preschool children
While there is substantial evidence that adults who violate gender stereotypes often face backlash (i.e. social and economic penalties), less is known about the nature of gender stereotypes for young children, and the penalties that children may face for violating them. We conducted three experiment...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Public Library of Science
2018
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5890994/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29630651 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0195503 |
_version_ | 1783312943317778432 |
---|---|
author | Sullivan, Jessica Moss-Racusin, Corinne Lopez, Michael Williams, Katherine |
author_facet | Sullivan, Jessica Moss-Racusin, Corinne Lopez, Michael Williams, Katherine |
author_sort | Sullivan, Jessica |
collection | PubMed |
description | While there is substantial evidence that adults who violate gender stereotypes often face backlash (i.e. social and economic penalties), less is known about the nature of gender stereotypes for young children, and the penalties that children may face for violating them. We conducted three experiments, with over 2000 adults from the US, to better understand the content and consequences of adults’ gender stereotypes for young children. In Experiment 1, we tested which characteristics adults (N = 635) believed to be descriptive (i.e. typical), prescriptive (i.e. required), and proscriptive (i.e. forbidden) for preschool-aged boys and girls. Using the characteristics that were rated in Experiment 1, we then constructed vignettes that were either ‘masculine’ or ‘feminine’, and manipulated whether the vignettes were said to describe a boy or a girl. Experiment 2 (N = 697) revealed that adults rated stereotype-violating children as less likeable than their stereotype-conforming peers, and that this difference was more robust for boys than girls. Experiment 3 (N = 731) was a direct replication of Experiment 2, and revealed converging evidence of backlash against stereotype-violating children. In sum, our results suggest that even young children encounter backlash from adults for stereotype violations, and that these effects may be strongest for boys. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-5890994 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2018 |
publisher | Public Library of Science |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-58909942018-04-20 Backlash against gender stereotype-violating preschool children Sullivan, Jessica Moss-Racusin, Corinne Lopez, Michael Williams, Katherine PLoS One Research Article While there is substantial evidence that adults who violate gender stereotypes often face backlash (i.e. social and economic penalties), less is known about the nature of gender stereotypes for young children, and the penalties that children may face for violating them. We conducted three experiments, with over 2000 adults from the US, to better understand the content and consequences of adults’ gender stereotypes for young children. In Experiment 1, we tested which characteristics adults (N = 635) believed to be descriptive (i.e. typical), prescriptive (i.e. required), and proscriptive (i.e. forbidden) for preschool-aged boys and girls. Using the characteristics that were rated in Experiment 1, we then constructed vignettes that were either ‘masculine’ or ‘feminine’, and manipulated whether the vignettes were said to describe a boy or a girl. Experiment 2 (N = 697) revealed that adults rated stereotype-violating children as less likeable than their stereotype-conforming peers, and that this difference was more robust for boys than girls. Experiment 3 (N = 731) was a direct replication of Experiment 2, and revealed converging evidence of backlash against stereotype-violating children. In sum, our results suggest that even young children encounter backlash from adults for stereotype violations, and that these effects may be strongest for boys. Public Library of Science 2018-04-09 /pmc/articles/PMC5890994/ /pubmed/29630651 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0195503 Text en © 2018 Sullivan et al http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. |
spellingShingle | Research Article Sullivan, Jessica Moss-Racusin, Corinne Lopez, Michael Williams, Katherine Backlash against gender stereotype-violating preschool children |
title | Backlash against gender stereotype-violating preschool children |
title_full | Backlash against gender stereotype-violating preschool children |
title_fullStr | Backlash against gender stereotype-violating preschool children |
title_full_unstemmed | Backlash against gender stereotype-violating preschool children |
title_short | Backlash against gender stereotype-violating preschool children |
title_sort | backlash against gender stereotype-violating preschool children |
topic | Research Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5890994/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29630651 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0195503 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT sullivanjessica backlashagainstgenderstereotypeviolatingpreschoolchildren AT mossracusincorinne backlashagainstgenderstereotypeviolatingpreschoolchildren AT lopezmichael backlashagainstgenderstereotypeviolatingpreschoolchildren AT williamskatherine backlashagainstgenderstereotypeviolatingpreschoolchildren |