Cargando…

The development of perceptual averaging: learning what to do, not just how to do it

The mature visual system condenses complex scenes into simple summary statistics (e.g., average size, location, orientation, etc.). However, children, often perform poorly on perceptual averaging tasks. Children's difficulties are typically thought to represent the suboptimal implementation of...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Jones, Pete R., Dekker, Tessa M.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: John Wiley and Sons Inc. 2017
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5947545/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28812307
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12584
_version_ 1783322391080861696
author Jones, Pete R.
Dekker, Tessa M.
author_facet Jones, Pete R.
Dekker, Tessa M.
author_sort Jones, Pete R.
collection PubMed
description The mature visual system condenses complex scenes into simple summary statistics (e.g., average size, location, orientation, etc.). However, children, often perform poorly on perceptual averaging tasks. Children's difficulties are typically thought to represent the suboptimal implementation of an adult‐like strategy. This paper examines another possibility: that children actually make decisions in a qualitatively different way to adults (optimal implementation of a non‐ideal strategy). Ninety children (6–7, 8–9, 10–11 years) and 30 adults were asked to locate the middle of randomly generated dot‐clouds. Nine plausible decision strategies were formulated, and each was fitted to observers' trial‐by‐trial response data (Reverse Correlation). When the number of visual elements was low (N < 6), children used a qualitatively different decision strategy from adults: appearing to “join up the dots” and locate the gravitational center of the enclosing shape. Given denser displays, both children and adults used an ideal strategy of arithmetically averaging individual points. Accounting for this difference in decision strategy explained 29% of children's lower precision. These findings suggest that children are not simply suboptimal at performing adult‐like computations, but may at times use sensible, but qualitatively different strategies to make perceptual judgments. Learning which strategy is best in which circumstance might be an important driving factor of perceptual development.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-5947545
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2017
publisher John Wiley and Sons Inc.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-59475452018-05-17 The development of perceptual averaging: learning what to do, not just how to do it Jones, Pete R. Dekker, Tessa M. Dev Sci Short Reports The mature visual system condenses complex scenes into simple summary statistics (e.g., average size, location, orientation, etc.). However, children, often perform poorly on perceptual averaging tasks. Children's difficulties are typically thought to represent the suboptimal implementation of an adult‐like strategy. This paper examines another possibility: that children actually make decisions in a qualitatively different way to adults (optimal implementation of a non‐ideal strategy). Ninety children (6–7, 8–9, 10–11 years) and 30 adults were asked to locate the middle of randomly generated dot‐clouds. Nine plausible decision strategies were formulated, and each was fitted to observers' trial‐by‐trial response data (Reverse Correlation). When the number of visual elements was low (N < 6), children used a qualitatively different decision strategy from adults: appearing to “join up the dots” and locate the gravitational center of the enclosing shape. Given denser displays, both children and adults used an ideal strategy of arithmetically averaging individual points. Accounting for this difference in decision strategy explained 29% of children's lower precision. These findings suggest that children are not simply suboptimal at performing adult‐like computations, but may at times use sensible, but qualitatively different strategies to make perceptual judgments. Learning which strategy is best in which circumstance might be an important driving factor of perceptual development. John Wiley and Sons Inc. 2017-08-15 2018-05 /pmc/articles/PMC5947545/ /pubmed/28812307 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12584 Text en ©2017 The Authors. Developmental Science Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd. This is an open access article under the terms of the http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
spellingShingle Short Reports
Jones, Pete R.
Dekker, Tessa M.
The development of perceptual averaging: learning what to do, not just how to do it
title The development of perceptual averaging: learning what to do, not just how to do it
title_full The development of perceptual averaging: learning what to do, not just how to do it
title_fullStr The development of perceptual averaging: learning what to do, not just how to do it
title_full_unstemmed The development of perceptual averaging: learning what to do, not just how to do it
title_short The development of perceptual averaging: learning what to do, not just how to do it
title_sort development of perceptual averaging: learning what to do, not just how to do it
topic Short Reports
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5947545/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28812307
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12584
work_keys_str_mv AT jonespeter thedevelopmentofperceptualaveraginglearningwhattodonotjusthowtodoit
AT dekkertessam thedevelopmentofperceptualaveraginglearningwhattodonotjusthowtodoit
AT jonespeter developmentofperceptualaveraginglearningwhattodonotjusthowtodoit
AT dekkertessam developmentofperceptualaveraginglearningwhattodonotjusthowtodoit