Cargando…

Thought probes during prospective memory encoding: Evidence for perfunctory processes

For nearly 50 years, psychologists have studied prospective memory, or the ability to execute delayed intentions. Yet, there remains a gap in understanding as to whether initial encoding of the intention must be elaborative and strategic, or whether some components of successful encoding can occur i...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Scullin, Michael K., McDaniel, Mark A., Dasse, Michelle N., Lee, Ji hae, Kurinec, Courtney A., Tami, Claudina, Krueger, Madison L.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Public Library of Science 2018
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5991366/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29874277
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198646
_version_ 1783329804442927104
author Scullin, Michael K.
McDaniel, Mark A.
Dasse, Michelle N.
Lee, Ji hae
Kurinec, Courtney A.
Tami, Claudina
Krueger, Madison L.
author_facet Scullin, Michael K.
McDaniel, Mark A.
Dasse, Michelle N.
Lee, Ji hae
Kurinec, Courtney A.
Tami, Claudina
Krueger, Madison L.
author_sort Scullin, Michael K.
collection PubMed
description For nearly 50 years, psychologists have studied prospective memory, or the ability to execute delayed intentions. Yet, there remains a gap in understanding as to whether initial encoding of the intention must be elaborative and strategic, or whether some components of successful encoding can occur in a perfunctory, transient manner. In eight studies (N = 680), we instructed participants to remember to press the Q key if they saw words representing fruits (cue) during an ongoing lexical decision task. They then typed what they were thinking and responded whether they encoded fruits as a general category, as specific exemplars, or hardly thought about it at all. Consistent with the perfunctory view, participants often reported mind wandering (42.9%) and hardly thinking about the prospective memory task (22.5%). Even though participants were given a general category cue, many participants generated specific category exemplars (34.5%). Bayesian analyses of encoding durations indicated that specific exemplars came to mind in a perfunctory manner rather than via strategic, elaborative mechanisms. Few participants correctly guessed the research hypotheses and changing from fruit category cues to initial-letter cues eliminated reports of specific exemplar generation, thereby arguing against demand characteristics in the thought probe procedure. In a final experiment, encoding duration was unrelated to prospective memory performance; however, specific-exemplar encoders outperformed general-category encoders with no ongoing task monitoring costs. Our findings reveal substantial variability in intention encoding, and demonstrate that some components of prospective memory encoding can be done “in passing.”
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-5991366
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2018
publisher Public Library of Science
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-59913662018-06-08 Thought probes during prospective memory encoding: Evidence for perfunctory processes Scullin, Michael K. McDaniel, Mark A. Dasse, Michelle N. Lee, Ji hae Kurinec, Courtney A. Tami, Claudina Krueger, Madison L. PLoS One Research Article For nearly 50 years, psychologists have studied prospective memory, or the ability to execute delayed intentions. Yet, there remains a gap in understanding as to whether initial encoding of the intention must be elaborative and strategic, or whether some components of successful encoding can occur in a perfunctory, transient manner. In eight studies (N = 680), we instructed participants to remember to press the Q key if they saw words representing fruits (cue) during an ongoing lexical decision task. They then typed what they were thinking and responded whether they encoded fruits as a general category, as specific exemplars, or hardly thought about it at all. Consistent with the perfunctory view, participants often reported mind wandering (42.9%) and hardly thinking about the prospective memory task (22.5%). Even though participants were given a general category cue, many participants generated specific category exemplars (34.5%). Bayesian analyses of encoding durations indicated that specific exemplars came to mind in a perfunctory manner rather than via strategic, elaborative mechanisms. Few participants correctly guessed the research hypotheses and changing from fruit category cues to initial-letter cues eliminated reports of specific exemplar generation, thereby arguing against demand characteristics in the thought probe procedure. In a final experiment, encoding duration was unrelated to prospective memory performance; however, specific-exemplar encoders outperformed general-category encoders with no ongoing task monitoring costs. Our findings reveal substantial variability in intention encoding, and demonstrate that some components of prospective memory encoding can be done “in passing.” Public Library of Science 2018-06-06 /pmc/articles/PMC5991366/ /pubmed/29874277 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198646 Text en © 2018 Scullin et al http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
spellingShingle Research Article
Scullin, Michael K.
McDaniel, Mark A.
Dasse, Michelle N.
Lee, Ji hae
Kurinec, Courtney A.
Tami, Claudina
Krueger, Madison L.
Thought probes during prospective memory encoding: Evidence for perfunctory processes
title Thought probes during prospective memory encoding: Evidence for perfunctory processes
title_full Thought probes during prospective memory encoding: Evidence for perfunctory processes
title_fullStr Thought probes during prospective memory encoding: Evidence for perfunctory processes
title_full_unstemmed Thought probes during prospective memory encoding: Evidence for perfunctory processes
title_short Thought probes during prospective memory encoding: Evidence for perfunctory processes
title_sort thought probes during prospective memory encoding: evidence for perfunctory processes
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5991366/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29874277
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198646
work_keys_str_mv AT scullinmichaelk thoughtprobesduringprospectivememoryencodingevidenceforperfunctoryprocesses
AT mcdanielmarka thoughtprobesduringprospectivememoryencodingevidenceforperfunctoryprocesses
AT dassemichellen thoughtprobesduringprospectivememoryencodingevidenceforperfunctoryprocesses
AT leejihae thoughtprobesduringprospectivememoryencodingevidenceforperfunctoryprocesses
AT kurineccourtneya thoughtprobesduringprospectivememoryencodingevidenceforperfunctoryprocesses
AT tamiclaudina thoughtprobesduringprospectivememoryencodingevidenceforperfunctoryprocesses
AT kruegermadisonl thoughtprobesduringprospectivememoryencodingevidenceforperfunctoryprocesses