Cargando…

Visual approach computation in feeding hoverflies

On warm sunny days, female hoverflies are often observed feeding from a wide range of wild and cultivated flowers. In doing so, hoverflies serve a vital role as alternative pollinators, and are suggested to be the most important pollinators after bees and bumblebees. Unless the flower hoverflies are...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Thyselius, Malin, Gonzalez-Bellido, Paloma T., Wardill, Trevor J., Nordström, Karin
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: The Company of Biologists Ltd 2018
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5992577/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29720383
http://dx.doi.org/10.1242/jeb.177162
_version_ 1783330052568514560
author Thyselius, Malin
Gonzalez-Bellido, Paloma T.
Wardill, Trevor J.
Nordström, Karin
author_facet Thyselius, Malin
Gonzalez-Bellido, Paloma T.
Wardill, Trevor J.
Nordström, Karin
author_sort Thyselius, Malin
collection PubMed
description On warm sunny days, female hoverflies are often observed feeding from a wide range of wild and cultivated flowers. In doing so, hoverflies serve a vital role as alternative pollinators, and are suggested to be the most important pollinators after bees and bumblebees. Unless the flower hoverflies are feeding from is large, they do not readily share the space with other insects, but instead opt to leave if another insect approaches. We used high-speed videography followed by 3D reconstruction of flight trajectories to quantify how female Eristalis hoverflies respond to approaching bees, wasps and two different hoverfly species. We found that, in 94% of the interactions, the occupant female left the flower when approached by another insect. We found that compared with spontaneous take-offs, the occupant hoverfly's escape response was performed at ∼3 times higher speed (spontaneous take-off at 0.2±0.05 m s(−1) compared with 0.55±0.08 m s(−1) when approached by another Eristalis). The hoverflies tended to take off upward and forward, while taking the incomer's approach angle into account. Intriguingly, we found that, when approached by wasps, the occupant Eristalis took off at a higher speed and when the wasp was further away. This suggests that feeding hoverflies may be able to distinguish these predators, demanding impressive visual capabilities. Our results, including quantification of the visual information available before occupant take-off, provide important insight into how freely behaving hoverflies perform escape responses from competitors and predators (e.g. wasps) in the wild.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-5992577
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2018
publisher The Company of Biologists Ltd
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-59925772018-07-05 Visual approach computation in feeding hoverflies Thyselius, Malin Gonzalez-Bellido, Paloma T. Wardill, Trevor J. Nordström, Karin J Exp Biol Research Article On warm sunny days, female hoverflies are often observed feeding from a wide range of wild and cultivated flowers. In doing so, hoverflies serve a vital role as alternative pollinators, and are suggested to be the most important pollinators after bees and bumblebees. Unless the flower hoverflies are feeding from is large, they do not readily share the space with other insects, but instead opt to leave if another insect approaches. We used high-speed videography followed by 3D reconstruction of flight trajectories to quantify how female Eristalis hoverflies respond to approaching bees, wasps and two different hoverfly species. We found that, in 94% of the interactions, the occupant female left the flower when approached by another insect. We found that compared with spontaneous take-offs, the occupant hoverfly's escape response was performed at ∼3 times higher speed (spontaneous take-off at 0.2±0.05 m s(−1) compared with 0.55±0.08 m s(−1) when approached by another Eristalis). The hoverflies tended to take off upward and forward, while taking the incomer's approach angle into account. Intriguingly, we found that, when approached by wasps, the occupant Eristalis took off at a higher speed and when the wasp was further away. This suggests that feeding hoverflies may be able to distinguish these predators, demanding impressive visual capabilities. Our results, including quantification of the visual information available before occupant take-off, provide important insight into how freely behaving hoverflies perform escape responses from competitors and predators (e.g. wasps) in the wild. The Company of Biologists Ltd 2018-05-15 2018-05-22 /pmc/articles/PMC5992577/ /pubmed/29720383 http://dx.doi.org/10.1242/jeb.177162 Text en © 2018. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.
spellingShingle Research Article
Thyselius, Malin
Gonzalez-Bellido, Paloma T.
Wardill, Trevor J.
Nordström, Karin
Visual approach computation in feeding hoverflies
title Visual approach computation in feeding hoverflies
title_full Visual approach computation in feeding hoverflies
title_fullStr Visual approach computation in feeding hoverflies
title_full_unstemmed Visual approach computation in feeding hoverflies
title_short Visual approach computation in feeding hoverflies
title_sort visual approach computation in feeding hoverflies
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5992577/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29720383
http://dx.doi.org/10.1242/jeb.177162
work_keys_str_mv AT thyseliusmalin visualapproachcomputationinfeedinghoverflies
AT gonzalezbellidopalomat visualapproachcomputationinfeedinghoverflies
AT wardilltrevorj visualapproachcomputationinfeedinghoverflies
AT nordstromkarin visualapproachcomputationinfeedinghoverflies