Cargando…
Intermediate frequency of aversive conditioning best restores wariness in habituated elk (Cervus canadensis)
In protected areas around the world, wildlife habituate to humans and human infrastructure, potentially resulting in human-wildlife conflict, and leading to trophic disruptions through excess herbivory and disconnection of predators from prey. For large species that threaten human safety, wildlife m...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Public Library of Science
2018
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6016931/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29940021 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199216 |
_version_ | 1783334634769088512 |
---|---|
author | Found, Rob Kloppers, Elsabé L. Hurd, Thomas E. St. Clair, Colleen Cassady |
author_facet | Found, Rob Kloppers, Elsabé L. Hurd, Thomas E. St. Clair, Colleen Cassady |
author_sort | Found, Rob |
collection | PubMed |
description | In protected areas around the world, wildlife habituate to humans and human infrastructure, potentially resulting in human-wildlife conflict, and leading to trophic disruptions through excess herbivory and disconnection of predators from prey. For large species that threaten human safety, wildlife managers sometimes attempt to reverse habituation with aversive conditioning. This technique associates people as a conditioned stimulus with a negative, unconditioned stimulus, such as pain or fright, to increase wariness and prevent the need for lethal wildlife management. Resistance to aversive conditioning by some habituated individuals often results in more frequent conditioning events by managers, but there are few studies of conditioning frequency with which to evaluate the usefulness of this management response. We evaluated the effect of conditioning frequency on the wariness of elk (Cervus canadensis) by subjecting marked individuals to predator-resembling chases by people over a period of three months. In that time, animals were subjected to conditioning a total of 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, or 9 times which we analyzed as both an ordinal variable and a binary one divided into low (3–5) and high (6–9) conditioning frequencies. We measured wariness before, during, and after the conditioning period using flight response distances from an approaching researcher. During the conditioning period, overall wariness increased significantly for elk in both treatment groups, although the increase was significantly greater in individuals subjected to high conditioning frequencies. However in the post-conditioning period, wariness gains also declined most in the high-frequency group, equating to more rapid extinction of learned behaviour. Across all treatment frequencies, rapid changes in flight responses also characterized the individuals with the lowest wariness at the beginning of the study period, suggesting that individuals with greater behavioural flexibility are more likely to habituate to both people and their attempts to change wariness via aversive conditioning. Together, our results imply that aversive conditioning may be most effective at intermediate frequencies and that its utility might be further increased with proactive assessment of individual personalities in habituated wildlife. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-6016931 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2018 |
publisher | Public Library of Science |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-60169312018-07-07 Intermediate frequency of aversive conditioning best restores wariness in habituated elk (Cervus canadensis) Found, Rob Kloppers, Elsabé L. Hurd, Thomas E. St. Clair, Colleen Cassady PLoS One Research Article In protected areas around the world, wildlife habituate to humans and human infrastructure, potentially resulting in human-wildlife conflict, and leading to trophic disruptions through excess herbivory and disconnection of predators from prey. For large species that threaten human safety, wildlife managers sometimes attempt to reverse habituation with aversive conditioning. This technique associates people as a conditioned stimulus with a negative, unconditioned stimulus, such as pain or fright, to increase wariness and prevent the need for lethal wildlife management. Resistance to aversive conditioning by some habituated individuals often results in more frequent conditioning events by managers, but there are few studies of conditioning frequency with which to evaluate the usefulness of this management response. We evaluated the effect of conditioning frequency on the wariness of elk (Cervus canadensis) by subjecting marked individuals to predator-resembling chases by people over a period of three months. In that time, animals were subjected to conditioning a total of 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, or 9 times which we analyzed as both an ordinal variable and a binary one divided into low (3–5) and high (6–9) conditioning frequencies. We measured wariness before, during, and after the conditioning period using flight response distances from an approaching researcher. During the conditioning period, overall wariness increased significantly for elk in both treatment groups, although the increase was significantly greater in individuals subjected to high conditioning frequencies. However in the post-conditioning period, wariness gains also declined most in the high-frequency group, equating to more rapid extinction of learned behaviour. Across all treatment frequencies, rapid changes in flight responses also characterized the individuals with the lowest wariness at the beginning of the study period, suggesting that individuals with greater behavioural flexibility are more likely to habituate to both people and their attempts to change wariness via aversive conditioning. Together, our results imply that aversive conditioning may be most effective at intermediate frequencies and that its utility might be further increased with proactive assessment of individual personalities in habituated wildlife. Public Library of Science 2018-06-25 /pmc/articles/PMC6016931/ /pubmed/29940021 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199216 Text en © 2018 Found et al http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. |
spellingShingle | Research Article Found, Rob Kloppers, Elsabé L. Hurd, Thomas E. St. Clair, Colleen Cassady Intermediate frequency of aversive conditioning best restores wariness in habituated elk (Cervus canadensis) |
title | Intermediate frequency of aversive conditioning best restores wariness in habituated elk (Cervus canadensis) |
title_full | Intermediate frequency of aversive conditioning best restores wariness in habituated elk (Cervus canadensis) |
title_fullStr | Intermediate frequency of aversive conditioning best restores wariness in habituated elk (Cervus canadensis) |
title_full_unstemmed | Intermediate frequency of aversive conditioning best restores wariness in habituated elk (Cervus canadensis) |
title_short | Intermediate frequency of aversive conditioning best restores wariness in habituated elk (Cervus canadensis) |
title_sort | intermediate frequency of aversive conditioning best restores wariness in habituated elk (cervus canadensis) |
topic | Research Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6016931/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29940021 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199216 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT foundrob intermediatefrequencyofaversiveconditioningbestrestoreswarinessinhabituatedelkcervuscanadensis AT klopperselsabel intermediatefrequencyofaversiveconditioningbestrestoreswarinessinhabituatedelkcervuscanadensis AT hurdthomase intermediatefrequencyofaversiveconditioningbestrestoreswarinessinhabituatedelkcervuscanadensis AT stclaircolleencassady intermediatefrequencyofaversiveconditioningbestrestoreswarinessinhabituatedelkcervuscanadensis |