Cargando…

A Mental Timeline for Duration From the Age of 5 Years Old

Both time and number can be represented in spatial terms. While their representation in terms of spatial magnitude (distance or size) might be innate, their representation in terms of spatial position (left/right or up/down) is acquired. In Western culture, the mental timeline represents past/future...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Coull, Jennifer T., Johnson, Katherine A., Droit-Volet, Sylvie
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Frontiers Media S.A. 2018
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6048416/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30042709
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01155
_version_ 1783340106673815552
author Coull, Jennifer T.
Johnson, Katherine A.
Droit-Volet, Sylvie
author_facet Coull, Jennifer T.
Johnson, Katherine A.
Droit-Volet, Sylvie
author_sort Coull, Jennifer T.
collection PubMed
description Both time and number can be represented in spatial terms. While their representation in terms of spatial magnitude (distance or size) might be innate, their representation in terms of spatial position (left/right or up/down) is acquired. In Western culture, the mental timeline represents past/future events or short/long duration on the left/right sides of space, respectively. We conducted two developmental studies to pinpoint the age at which the mental timeline for duration begins to be acquired. Children (aged 5–6, 8, or 10 years old) and adults performed temporal bisection tasks in which relative spatial position (left/right) was manipulated by either arrow direction (Experiment 1) and/or lateralized stimulus location (Experiments 1 and 2). Results first confirmed previous findings that the symbolic representation of spatial position conveyed by arrow stimuli influences the perception of duration in older children. Both 8 and 10 year olds judged the duration of leftward arrows to be shorter than that of rightward arrows. We also showed for the first time that as long as position is manipulated in a non-symbolic way by the visual eccentricity of the stimuli, then even 5–6 year olds’ perception of duration is influenced by spatial position. These children judged the duration of left-lateralized stimuli to be shorter than that of either right-lateralized or centrally located stimuli. These data are consistent with the use of a mental timeline for stimulus duration from the age of 5 years old, with short duration being represented on the left side of space and long duration on the right. Nevertheless, the way in which left and right were manipulated determined the age at which spatial position influenced duration judgment: physical spatial location influenced duration perception from the age of 5 years old whereas arrow direction influenced it from the age of 8. This age-related dissociation may reflect distinct developmental trajectories of automatic versus voluntary spatial attentional mechanisms and, more generally highlights the importance of accounting for attentional ability when interpreting results of duration judgment tasks.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-6048416
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2018
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-60484162018-07-24 A Mental Timeline for Duration From the Age of 5 Years Old Coull, Jennifer T. Johnson, Katherine A. Droit-Volet, Sylvie Front Psychol Psychology Both time and number can be represented in spatial terms. While their representation in terms of spatial magnitude (distance or size) might be innate, their representation in terms of spatial position (left/right or up/down) is acquired. In Western culture, the mental timeline represents past/future events or short/long duration on the left/right sides of space, respectively. We conducted two developmental studies to pinpoint the age at which the mental timeline for duration begins to be acquired. Children (aged 5–6, 8, or 10 years old) and adults performed temporal bisection tasks in which relative spatial position (left/right) was manipulated by either arrow direction (Experiment 1) and/or lateralized stimulus location (Experiments 1 and 2). Results first confirmed previous findings that the symbolic representation of spatial position conveyed by arrow stimuli influences the perception of duration in older children. Both 8 and 10 year olds judged the duration of leftward arrows to be shorter than that of rightward arrows. We also showed for the first time that as long as position is manipulated in a non-symbolic way by the visual eccentricity of the stimuli, then even 5–6 year olds’ perception of duration is influenced by spatial position. These children judged the duration of left-lateralized stimuli to be shorter than that of either right-lateralized or centrally located stimuli. These data are consistent with the use of a mental timeline for stimulus duration from the age of 5 years old, with short duration being represented on the left side of space and long duration on the right. Nevertheless, the way in which left and right were manipulated determined the age at which spatial position influenced duration judgment: physical spatial location influenced duration perception from the age of 5 years old whereas arrow direction influenced it from the age of 8. This age-related dissociation may reflect distinct developmental trajectories of automatic versus voluntary spatial attentional mechanisms and, more generally highlights the importance of accounting for attentional ability when interpreting results of duration judgment tasks. Frontiers Media S.A. 2018-07-10 /pmc/articles/PMC6048416/ /pubmed/30042709 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01155 Text en Copyright © 2018 Coull, Johnson and Droit-Volet. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
spellingShingle Psychology
Coull, Jennifer T.
Johnson, Katherine A.
Droit-Volet, Sylvie
A Mental Timeline for Duration From the Age of 5 Years Old
title A Mental Timeline for Duration From the Age of 5 Years Old
title_full A Mental Timeline for Duration From the Age of 5 Years Old
title_fullStr A Mental Timeline for Duration From the Age of 5 Years Old
title_full_unstemmed A Mental Timeline for Duration From the Age of 5 Years Old
title_short A Mental Timeline for Duration From the Age of 5 Years Old
title_sort mental timeline for duration from the age of 5 years old
topic Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6048416/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30042709
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01155
work_keys_str_mv AT coulljennifert amentaltimelinefordurationfromtheageof5yearsold
AT johnsonkatherinea amentaltimelinefordurationfromtheageof5yearsold
AT droitvoletsylvie amentaltimelinefordurationfromtheageof5yearsold
AT coulljennifert mentaltimelinefordurationfromtheageof5yearsold
AT johnsonkatherinea mentaltimelinefordurationfromtheageof5yearsold
AT droitvoletsylvie mentaltimelinefordurationfromtheageof5yearsold