Cargando…
Predator odour but not TMT induces 22-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats that lead to defensive behaviours in conspecifics upon replay
Predator odours induce defensive behaviour in prey animals such as rats. The present study investigated (1) whether laboratory rats exposed to predator odours emit 22-kHz calls which may have an alarming function and (2) whether playback of such calls induces behavioural changes in conspecifics. For...
Autores principales: | , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Nature Publishing Group UK
2018
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6056510/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30038341 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-28927-4 |
_version_ | 1783341351810629632 |
---|---|
author | Fendt, Markus Brosch, Marcel Wernecke, Kerstin E. A. Willadsen, Maria Wöhr, Markus |
author_facet | Fendt, Markus Brosch, Marcel Wernecke, Kerstin E. A. Willadsen, Maria Wöhr, Markus |
author_sort | Fendt, Markus |
collection | PubMed |
description | Predator odours induce defensive behaviour in prey animals such as rats. The present study investigated (1) whether laboratory rats exposed to predator odours emit 22-kHz calls which may have an alarming function and (2) whether playback of such calls induces behavioural changes in conspecifics. For this, Sprague-Dawley rats were exposed to samples of fox and lion urine, as well as to the synthetic predator odour TMT. Despite that all odours induced defensive behaviour, only predator urine samples but not TMT were able to induce 22-kHz calls in a few rats. In a second experiment, naive rats were exposed to playback presentations of the 22-kHz calls recorded in the first experiment, as well as to phase-scrambled and frequency-shifted control stimuli. Low intensity playback presentations led to a reduction of locomotor activity during the presentation of the 22-kHz calls but not of the control stimuli. This effect was less specific under high intensity conditions. Taken together the present findings show that natural predator odours are able to induce emission of 22-kHz calls in rats and support the hypothesis that these calls have an alarming function. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-6056510 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2018 |
publisher | Nature Publishing Group UK |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-60565102018-07-30 Predator odour but not TMT induces 22-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats that lead to defensive behaviours in conspecifics upon replay Fendt, Markus Brosch, Marcel Wernecke, Kerstin E. A. Willadsen, Maria Wöhr, Markus Sci Rep Article Predator odours induce defensive behaviour in prey animals such as rats. The present study investigated (1) whether laboratory rats exposed to predator odours emit 22-kHz calls which may have an alarming function and (2) whether playback of such calls induces behavioural changes in conspecifics. For this, Sprague-Dawley rats were exposed to samples of fox and lion urine, as well as to the synthetic predator odour TMT. Despite that all odours induced defensive behaviour, only predator urine samples but not TMT were able to induce 22-kHz calls in a few rats. In a second experiment, naive rats were exposed to playback presentations of the 22-kHz calls recorded in the first experiment, as well as to phase-scrambled and frequency-shifted control stimuli. Low intensity playback presentations led to a reduction of locomotor activity during the presentation of the 22-kHz calls but not of the control stimuli. This effect was less specific under high intensity conditions. Taken together the present findings show that natural predator odours are able to induce emission of 22-kHz calls in rats and support the hypothesis that these calls have an alarming function. Nature Publishing Group UK 2018-07-23 /pmc/articles/PMC6056510/ /pubmed/30038341 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-28927-4 Text en © The Author(s) 2018 Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. |
spellingShingle | Article Fendt, Markus Brosch, Marcel Wernecke, Kerstin E. A. Willadsen, Maria Wöhr, Markus Predator odour but not TMT induces 22-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats that lead to defensive behaviours in conspecifics upon replay |
title | Predator odour but not TMT induces 22-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats that lead to defensive behaviours in conspecifics upon replay |
title_full | Predator odour but not TMT induces 22-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats that lead to defensive behaviours in conspecifics upon replay |
title_fullStr | Predator odour but not TMT induces 22-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats that lead to defensive behaviours in conspecifics upon replay |
title_full_unstemmed | Predator odour but not TMT induces 22-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats that lead to defensive behaviours in conspecifics upon replay |
title_short | Predator odour but not TMT induces 22-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats that lead to defensive behaviours in conspecifics upon replay |
title_sort | predator odour but not tmt induces 22-khz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats that lead to defensive behaviours in conspecifics upon replay |
topic | Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6056510/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30038341 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-28927-4 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT fendtmarkus predatorodourbutnottmtinduces22khzultrasonicvocalizationsinratsthatleadtodefensivebehavioursinconspecificsuponreplay AT broschmarcel predatorodourbutnottmtinduces22khzultrasonicvocalizationsinratsthatleadtodefensivebehavioursinconspecificsuponreplay AT werneckekerstinea predatorodourbutnottmtinduces22khzultrasonicvocalizationsinratsthatleadtodefensivebehavioursinconspecificsuponreplay AT willadsenmaria predatorodourbutnottmtinduces22khzultrasonicvocalizationsinratsthatleadtodefensivebehavioursinconspecificsuponreplay AT wohrmarkus predatorodourbutnottmtinduces22khzultrasonicvocalizationsinratsthatleadtodefensivebehavioursinconspecificsuponreplay |