Cargando…

Multiple time scales of the ventriloquism aftereffect

The ventriloquism aftereffect (VAE) refers to a shift in auditory spatial perception following exposure to a spatial disparity between auditory and visual stimuli. The VAE has been previously measured on two distinct time scales. Hundreds or thousands of exposures to a an audio-visual spatial dispar...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Bosen, Adam K., Fleming, Justin T., Allen, Paul D., O’Neill, William E., Paige, Gary D.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Public Library of Science 2018
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6070234/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30067790
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0200930
_version_ 1783343641253642240
author Bosen, Adam K.
Fleming, Justin T.
Allen, Paul D.
O’Neill, William E.
Paige, Gary D.
author_facet Bosen, Adam K.
Fleming, Justin T.
Allen, Paul D.
O’Neill, William E.
Paige, Gary D.
author_sort Bosen, Adam K.
collection PubMed
description The ventriloquism aftereffect (VAE) refers to a shift in auditory spatial perception following exposure to a spatial disparity between auditory and visual stimuli. The VAE has been previously measured on two distinct time scales. Hundreds or thousands of exposures to a an audio-visual spatial disparity produces enduring VAE that persists after exposure ceases. Exposure to a single audio-visual spatial disparity produces immediate VAE that decays over seconds. To determine if these phenomena are two extremes of a continuum or represent distinct processes, we conducted an experiment with normal hearing listeners that measured VAE in response to a repeated, constant audio-visual disparity sequence, both immediately after exposure to each audio-visual disparity and after the end of the sequence. In each experimental session, subjects were exposed to sequences of auditory and visual targets that were constantly offset by +8° or −8° in azimuth from one another, then localized auditory targets presented in isolation following each sequence. Eye position was controlled throughout the experiment, to avoid the effects of gaze on auditory localization. In contrast to other studies that did not control eye position, we found both a large shift in auditory perception that decayed rapidly after each AV disparity exposure, along with a gradual shift in auditory perception that grew over time and persisted after exposure to the AV disparity ceased. We modeled the temporal and spatial properties of the measured auditory shifts using grey box nonlinear system identification, and found that two models could explain the data equally well. In the power model, the temporal decay of the ventriloquism aftereffect was modeled with a power law relationship. This causes an initial rapid drop in auditory shift, followed by a long tail which accumulates with repeated exposure to audio-visual disparity. In the double exponential model, two separate processes were required to explain the data, one which accumulated and decayed exponentially and the other which slowly integrated over time. Both models fit the data best when the spatial spread of the ventriloquism aftereffect was limited to a window around the location of the audio-visual disparity. We directly compare the predictions made by each model, and suggest additional measurements that could help distinguish which model best describes the mechanisms underlying the VAE.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-6070234
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2018
publisher Public Library of Science
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-60702342018-08-09 Multiple time scales of the ventriloquism aftereffect Bosen, Adam K. Fleming, Justin T. Allen, Paul D. O’Neill, William E. Paige, Gary D. PLoS One Research Article The ventriloquism aftereffect (VAE) refers to a shift in auditory spatial perception following exposure to a spatial disparity between auditory and visual stimuli. The VAE has been previously measured on two distinct time scales. Hundreds or thousands of exposures to a an audio-visual spatial disparity produces enduring VAE that persists after exposure ceases. Exposure to a single audio-visual spatial disparity produces immediate VAE that decays over seconds. To determine if these phenomena are two extremes of a continuum or represent distinct processes, we conducted an experiment with normal hearing listeners that measured VAE in response to a repeated, constant audio-visual disparity sequence, both immediately after exposure to each audio-visual disparity and after the end of the sequence. In each experimental session, subjects were exposed to sequences of auditory and visual targets that were constantly offset by +8° or −8° in azimuth from one another, then localized auditory targets presented in isolation following each sequence. Eye position was controlled throughout the experiment, to avoid the effects of gaze on auditory localization. In contrast to other studies that did not control eye position, we found both a large shift in auditory perception that decayed rapidly after each AV disparity exposure, along with a gradual shift in auditory perception that grew over time and persisted after exposure to the AV disparity ceased. We modeled the temporal and spatial properties of the measured auditory shifts using grey box nonlinear system identification, and found that two models could explain the data equally well. In the power model, the temporal decay of the ventriloquism aftereffect was modeled with a power law relationship. This causes an initial rapid drop in auditory shift, followed by a long tail which accumulates with repeated exposure to audio-visual disparity. In the double exponential model, two separate processes were required to explain the data, one which accumulated and decayed exponentially and the other which slowly integrated over time. Both models fit the data best when the spatial spread of the ventriloquism aftereffect was limited to a window around the location of the audio-visual disparity. We directly compare the predictions made by each model, and suggest additional measurements that could help distinguish which model best describes the mechanisms underlying the VAE. Public Library of Science 2018-08-01 /pmc/articles/PMC6070234/ /pubmed/30067790 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0200930 Text en © 2018 Bosen et al http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
spellingShingle Research Article
Bosen, Adam K.
Fleming, Justin T.
Allen, Paul D.
O’Neill, William E.
Paige, Gary D.
Multiple time scales of the ventriloquism aftereffect
title Multiple time scales of the ventriloquism aftereffect
title_full Multiple time scales of the ventriloquism aftereffect
title_fullStr Multiple time scales of the ventriloquism aftereffect
title_full_unstemmed Multiple time scales of the ventriloquism aftereffect
title_short Multiple time scales of the ventriloquism aftereffect
title_sort multiple time scales of the ventriloquism aftereffect
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6070234/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30067790
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0200930
work_keys_str_mv AT bosenadamk multipletimescalesoftheventriloquismaftereffect
AT flemingjustint multipletimescalesoftheventriloquismaftereffect
AT allenpauld multipletimescalesoftheventriloquismaftereffect
AT oneillwilliame multipletimescalesoftheventriloquismaftereffect
AT paigegaryd multipletimescalesoftheventriloquismaftereffect