Cargando…
Memory bias in observer-performance literature
The objective of our study was to determine how authors of published observer–performance experiments dealt with memory bias in study design. We searched American Journal of Roentgenology online and Radiology using “observer study” and “observer performance.” We included articles from 1970 or later...
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Society of Photo-Optical Instrumentation Engineers
2018
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6152535/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30840725 http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/1.JMI.5.3.031412 |
_version_ | 1783357378295496704 |
---|---|
author | Haygood, Tamara Miner Smith, Samantha Sun, Jia |
author_facet | Haygood, Tamara Miner Smith, Samantha Sun, Jia |
author_sort | Haygood, Tamara Miner |
collection | PubMed |
description | The objective of our study was to determine how authors of published observer–performance experiments dealt with memory bias in study design. We searched American Journal of Roentgenology online and Radiology using “observer study” and “observer performance.” We included articles from 1970 or later that reported an observer performance experiment using human observers. We recorded the methods used by the authors to order presentation of the conditions being tested and images within sets for viewing. We recorded use and length of any time gap between viewings. We included 110 experiments. Forty-five used methods not subject to memory bias. Of 68 remaining experiments, 30 (44.1%) ordered the viewing of tested conditions to decrease memory bias. Fifteen (22.1%) ordered the tested conditions in ways that may create memory bias. Eleven (16.2%) intermixed the tested conditions. Forty-three (63.2%) used random or pseudorandom ordering of images within sets. Forty-six (67.6%) used a time gap (median 14 days) between viewings. Six (8.8%) did not use a time gap. Thirty-six (52.9%) did not indicate what methods they used in at least one studied parameter. Therefore, we conclude that 22.1% of the experiments could improve their methods of ordering tested conditions. Completeness of reporting could be improved by including more details regarding methods of ameliorating memory bias. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-6152535 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2018 |
publisher | Society of Photo-Optical Instrumentation Engineers |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-61525352019-09-24 Memory bias in observer-performance literature Haygood, Tamara Miner Smith, Samantha Sun, Jia J Med Imaging (Bellingham) Special Section on Medical Image Perceptions and Observer Performance The objective of our study was to determine how authors of published observer–performance experiments dealt with memory bias in study design. We searched American Journal of Roentgenology online and Radiology using “observer study” and “observer performance.” We included articles from 1970 or later that reported an observer performance experiment using human observers. We recorded the methods used by the authors to order presentation of the conditions being tested and images within sets for viewing. We recorded use and length of any time gap between viewings. We included 110 experiments. Forty-five used methods not subject to memory bias. Of 68 remaining experiments, 30 (44.1%) ordered the viewing of tested conditions to decrease memory bias. Fifteen (22.1%) ordered the tested conditions in ways that may create memory bias. Eleven (16.2%) intermixed the tested conditions. Forty-three (63.2%) used random or pseudorandom ordering of images within sets. Forty-six (67.6%) used a time gap (median 14 days) between viewings. Six (8.8%) did not use a time gap. Thirty-six (52.9%) did not indicate what methods they used in at least one studied parameter. Therefore, we conclude that 22.1% of the experiments could improve their methods of ordering tested conditions. Completeness of reporting could be improved by including more details regarding methods of ameliorating memory bias. Society of Photo-Optical Instrumentation Engineers 2018-09-24 2018-07 /pmc/articles/PMC6152535/ /pubmed/30840725 http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/1.JMI.5.3.031412 Text en © The Authors. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ Published by SPIE under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 Unported License. Distribution or reproduction of this work in whole or in part requires full attribution of the original publication, including its DOI. |
spellingShingle | Special Section on Medical Image Perceptions and Observer Performance Haygood, Tamara Miner Smith, Samantha Sun, Jia Memory bias in observer-performance literature |
title | Memory bias in observer-performance literature |
title_full | Memory bias in observer-performance literature |
title_fullStr | Memory bias in observer-performance literature |
title_full_unstemmed | Memory bias in observer-performance literature |
title_short | Memory bias in observer-performance literature |
title_sort | memory bias in observer-performance literature |
topic | Special Section on Medical Image Perceptions and Observer Performance |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6152535/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30840725 http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/1.JMI.5.3.031412 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT haygoodtamaraminer memorybiasinobserverperformanceliterature AT smithsamantha memorybiasinobserverperformanceliterature AT sunjia memorybiasinobserverperformanceliterature |