Cargando…
What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed nonwords?
Nonwords created by transposing two non-adjacent orthographic consonants (CONDISER) have been reported to produce more priming for their baseword (CONSIDER), and to be classified as a nonword less readily than nonwords created by transposing two orthographic vowels (CINSODER). We investigate the ori...
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
SAGE Publications
2017
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6159775/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28093020 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1271444 |
_version_ | 1783358654112595968 |
---|---|
author | Schubert, Teresa Kinoshita, Sachiko Norris, Dennis |
author_facet | Schubert, Teresa Kinoshita, Sachiko Norris, Dennis |
author_sort | Schubert, Teresa |
collection | PubMed |
description | Nonwords created by transposing two non-adjacent orthographic consonants (CONDISER) have been reported to produce more priming for their baseword (CONSIDER), and to be classified as a nonword less readily than nonwords created by transposing two orthographic vowels (CINSODER). We investigate the origin of this difference and its relevance for theories of letter position coding. In the unprimed versions of the lexical decision and same–different tasks, a consonant–vowel difference was found in the transposition condition, not when those letters are substituted (Experiment 1). We found that when transpositions involved the disruption of a consonant cluster (OPMITAL), reaction times were slowed compared to when transpositions involved only letters that are separated (CHOLOCATE; Experiment 2). As transpositions more frequently disrupt in consonant clusters than vowel clusters, this introduces a confound in studies investigating consonant and vowel transposition effects. Consistent with the idea that letter order is harder to resolve in clusters, the difference between consonants and vowels was eliminated when transpositions involve singleton consonants or vowels rather than those in clusters (Experiment 3). These results suggest that the precision of position coding does not differ between consonants and vowels, but that consonant–vowel status plays a role in structuring orthographic representations. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-6159775 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2017 |
publisher | SAGE Publications |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-61597752018-10-11 What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed nonwords? Schubert, Teresa Kinoshita, Sachiko Norris, Dennis Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) Original Articles Nonwords created by transposing two non-adjacent orthographic consonants (CONDISER) have been reported to produce more priming for their baseword (CONSIDER), and to be classified as a nonword less readily than nonwords created by transposing two orthographic vowels (CINSODER). We investigate the origin of this difference and its relevance for theories of letter position coding. In the unprimed versions of the lexical decision and same–different tasks, a consonant–vowel difference was found in the transposition condition, not when those letters are substituted (Experiment 1). We found that when transpositions involved the disruption of a consonant cluster (OPMITAL), reaction times were slowed compared to when transpositions involved only letters that are separated (CHOLOCATE; Experiment 2). As transpositions more frequently disrupt in consonant clusters than vowel clusters, this introduces a confound in studies investigating consonant and vowel transposition effects. Consistent with the idea that letter order is harder to resolve in clusters, the difference between consonants and vowels was eliminated when transpositions involve singleton consonants or vowels rather than those in clusters (Experiment 3). These results suggest that the precision of position coding does not differ between consonants and vowels, but that consonant–vowel status plays a role in structuring orthographic representations. SAGE Publications 2017-01-17 2018-03 /pmc/articles/PMC6159775/ /pubmed/28093020 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1271444 Text en © Experimental Psychology Society 2017 http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs 4.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work as published without adaptation or alteration, without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage). |
spellingShingle | Original Articles Schubert, Teresa Kinoshita, Sachiko Norris, Dennis What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed nonwords? |
title | What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed
nonwords? |
title_full | What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed
nonwords? |
title_fullStr | What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed
nonwords? |
title_full_unstemmed | What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed
nonwords? |
title_short | What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed
nonwords? |
title_sort | what causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed
nonwords? |
topic | Original Articles |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6159775/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28093020 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1271444 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT schubertteresa whatcausesthegreaterperceivedsimilarityofconsonanttransposednonwords AT kinoshitasachiko whatcausesthegreaterperceivedsimilarityofconsonanttransposednonwords AT norrisdennis whatcausesthegreaterperceivedsimilarityofconsonanttransposednonwords |