Cargando…

What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed nonwords?

Nonwords created by transposing two non-adjacent orthographic consonants (CONDISER) have been reported to produce more priming for their baseword (CONSIDER), and to be classified as a nonword less readily than nonwords created by transposing two orthographic vowels (CINSODER). We investigate the ori...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Schubert, Teresa, Kinoshita, Sachiko, Norris, Dennis
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: SAGE Publications 2017
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6159775/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28093020
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1271444
_version_ 1783358654112595968
author Schubert, Teresa
Kinoshita, Sachiko
Norris, Dennis
author_facet Schubert, Teresa
Kinoshita, Sachiko
Norris, Dennis
author_sort Schubert, Teresa
collection PubMed
description Nonwords created by transposing two non-adjacent orthographic consonants (CONDISER) have been reported to produce more priming for their baseword (CONSIDER), and to be classified as a nonword less readily than nonwords created by transposing two orthographic vowels (CINSODER). We investigate the origin of this difference and its relevance for theories of letter position coding. In the unprimed versions of the lexical decision and same–different tasks, a consonant–vowel difference was found in the transposition condition, not when those letters are substituted (Experiment 1). We found that when transpositions involved the disruption of a consonant cluster (OPMITAL), reaction times were slowed compared to when transpositions involved only letters that are separated (CHOLOCATE; Experiment 2). As transpositions more frequently disrupt in consonant clusters than vowel clusters, this introduces a confound in studies investigating consonant and vowel transposition effects. Consistent with the idea that letter order is harder to resolve in clusters, the difference between consonants and vowels was eliminated when transpositions involve singleton consonants or vowels rather than those in clusters (Experiment 3). These results suggest that the precision of position coding does not differ between consonants and vowels, but that consonant–vowel status plays a role in structuring orthographic representations.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-6159775
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2017
publisher SAGE Publications
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-61597752018-10-11 What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed nonwords? Schubert, Teresa Kinoshita, Sachiko Norris, Dennis Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) Original Articles Nonwords created by transposing two non-adjacent orthographic consonants (CONDISER) have been reported to produce more priming for their baseword (CONSIDER), and to be classified as a nonword less readily than nonwords created by transposing two orthographic vowels (CINSODER). We investigate the origin of this difference and its relevance for theories of letter position coding. In the unprimed versions of the lexical decision and same–different tasks, a consonant–vowel difference was found in the transposition condition, not when those letters are substituted (Experiment 1). We found that when transpositions involved the disruption of a consonant cluster (OPMITAL), reaction times were slowed compared to when transpositions involved only letters that are separated (CHOLOCATE; Experiment 2). As transpositions more frequently disrupt in consonant clusters than vowel clusters, this introduces a confound in studies investigating consonant and vowel transposition effects. Consistent with the idea that letter order is harder to resolve in clusters, the difference between consonants and vowels was eliminated when transpositions involve singleton consonants or vowels rather than those in clusters (Experiment 3). These results suggest that the precision of position coding does not differ between consonants and vowels, but that consonant–vowel status plays a role in structuring orthographic representations. SAGE Publications 2017-01-17 2018-03 /pmc/articles/PMC6159775/ /pubmed/28093020 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1271444 Text en © Experimental Psychology Society 2017 http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs 4.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work as published without adaptation or alteration, without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).
spellingShingle Original Articles
Schubert, Teresa
Kinoshita, Sachiko
Norris, Dennis
What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed nonwords?
title What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed nonwords?
title_full What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed nonwords?
title_fullStr What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed nonwords?
title_full_unstemmed What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed nonwords?
title_short What causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed nonwords?
title_sort what causes the greater perceived similarity of consonant-transposed nonwords?
topic Original Articles
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6159775/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28093020
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1271444
work_keys_str_mv AT schubertteresa whatcausesthegreaterperceivedsimilarityofconsonanttransposednonwords
AT kinoshitasachiko whatcausesthegreaterperceivedsimilarityofconsonanttransposednonwords
AT norrisdennis whatcausesthegreaterperceivedsimilarityofconsonanttransposednonwords