Cargando…

Evaluating the utility of camera traps in field studies of predation

Artificial prey techniques—wherein synthetic replicas of real organisms are placed in natural habitats—are widely used to study predation in the field. We investigated the extent to which videography could provide additional information to such studies. As a part of studies on aposematism and mimicr...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Akcali, Christopher K., Adán Pérez-Mendoza, Hibraim, Salazar-Valenzuela, David, Kikuchi, David W., Guayasamin, Juan M., Pfennig, David W.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: PeerJ Inc. 2019
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6394347/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30828493
http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.6487
_version_ 1783398878518706176
author Akcali, Christopher K.
Adán Pérez-Mendoza, Hibraim
Salazar-Valenzuela, David
Kikuchi, David W.
Guayasamin, Juan M.
Pfennig, David W.
author_facet Akcali, Christopher K.
Adán Pérez-Mendoza, Hibraim
Salazar-Valenzuela, David
Kikuchi, David W.
Guayasamin, Juan M.
Pfennig, David W.
author_sort Akcali, Christopher K.
collection PubMed
description Artificial prey techniques—wherein synthetic replicas of real organisms are placed in natural habitats—are widely used to study predation in the field. We investigated the extent to which videography could provide additional information to such studies. As a part of studies on aposematism and mimicry of coral snakes (Micrurus) and their mimics, observational data from 109 artificial snake prey were collected from video-recording camera traps in three locations in the Americas (terra firme forest, Tiputini Biodiversity Station, Ecuador; premontane wet forest, Nahá Reserve, Mexico; longleaf pine forest, Southeastern Coastal Plain, North Carolina, USA). During 1,536 camera days, a total of 268 observations of 20 putative snake predator species were recorded in the vicinity of artificial prey. Predators were observed to detect artificial prey 52 times, but only 21 attacks were recorded. Mammals were the most commonly recorded group of predators near replicas (243) and were responsible for most detections (48) and attacks (20). There was no difference between avian or mammalian predators in their probability of detecting replicas nor in their probability of attacking replicas after detecting them. Bite and beak marks left on clay replicas registered a higher ratio of avian:mammalian attacks than videos registered. Approximately 61.5% of artificial prey monitored with cameras remained undetected by predators throughout the duration of the experiments. Observational data collected from videos could provide more robust inferences on the relative fitness of different prey phenotypes, predator behavior, and the relative contribution of different predator species to selection on prey. However, we estimate that the level of predator activity necessary for the benefit of additional information that videos provide to be worth their financial costs is achieved in fewer than 20% of published artificial prey studies. Although we suggest future predation studies employing artificial prey to consider using videography as a tool to inspire new, more focused inquiry, the investment in camera traps is unlikely to be worth the expense for most artificial prey studies until the cost:benefit ratio decreases.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-6394347
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2019
publisher PeerJ Inc.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-63943472019-03-01 Evaluating the utility of camera traps in field studies of predation Akcali, Christopher K. Adán Pérez-Mendoza, Hibraim Salazar-Valenzuela, David Kikuchi, David W. Guayasamin, Juan M. Pfennig, David W. PeerJ Animal Behavior Artificial prey techniques—wherein synthetic replicas of real organisms are placed in natural habitats—are widely used to study predation in the field. We investigated the extent to which videography could provide additional information to such studies. As a part of studies on aposematism and mimicry of coral snakes (Micrurus) and their mimics, observational data from 109 artificial snake prey were collected from video-recording camera traps in three locations in the Americas (terra firme forest, Tiputini Biodiversity Station, Ecuador; premontane wet forest, Nahá Reserve, Mexico; longleaf pine forest, Southeastern Coastal Plain, North Carolina, USA). During 1,536 camera days, a total of 268 observations of 20 putative snake predator species were recorded in the vicinity of artificial prey. Predators were observed to detect artificial prey 52 times, but only 21 attacks were recorded. Mammals were the most commonly recorded group of predators near replicas (243) and were responsible for most detections (48) and attacks (20). There was no difference between avian or mammalian predators in their probability of detecting replicas nor in their probability of attacking replicas after detecting them. Bite and beak marks left on clay replicas registered a higher ratio of avian:mammalian attacks than videos registered. Approximately 61.5% of artificial prey monitored with cameras remained undetected by predators throughout the duration of the experiments. Observational data collected from videos could provide more robust inferences on the relative fitness of different prey phenotypes, predator behavior, and the relative contribution of different predator species to selection on prey. However, we estimate that the level of predator activity necessary for the benefit of additional information that videos provide to be worth their financial costs is achieved in fewer than 20% of published artificial prey studies. Although we suggest future predation studies employing artificial prey to consider using videography as a tool to inspire new, more focused inquiry, the investment in camera traps is unlikely to be worth the expense for most artificial prey studies until the cost:benefit ratio decreases. PeerJ Inc. 2019-02-25 /pmc/articles/PMC6394347/ /pubmed/30828493 http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.6487 Text en © 2019 Akcali et al. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, reproduction and adaptation in any medium and for any purpose provided that it is properly attributed. For attribution, the original author(s), title, publication source (PeerJ) and either DOI or URL of the article must be cited.
spellingShingle Animal Behavior
Akcali, Christopher K.
Adán Pérez-Mendoza, Hibraim
Salazar-Valenzuela, David
Kikuchi, David W.
Guayasamin, Juan M.
Pfennig, David W.
Evaluating the utility of camera traps in field studies of predation
title Evaluating the utility of camera traps in field studies of predation
title_full Evaluating the utility of camera traps in field studies of predation
title_fullStr Evaluating the utility of camera traps in field studies of predation
title_full_unstemmed Evaluating the utility of camera traps in field studies of predation
title_short Evaluating the utility of camera traps in field studies of predation
title_sort evaluating the utility of camera traps in field studies of predation
topic Animal Behavior
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6394347/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30828493
http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.6487
work_keys_str_mv AT akcalichristopherk evaluatingtheutilityofcameratrapsinfieldstudiesofpredation
AT adanperezmendozahibraim evaluatingtheutilityofcameratrapsinfieldstudiesofpredation
AT salazarvalenzueladavid evaluatingtheutilityofcameratrapsinfieldstudiesofpredation
AT kikuchidavidw evaluatingtheutilityofcameratrapsinfieldstudiesofpredation
AT guayasaminjuanm evaluatingtheutilityofcameratrapsinfieldstudiesofpredation
AT pfennigdavidw evaluatingtheutilityofcameratrapsinfieldstudiesofpredation