Cargando…
Phonological Ambiguity Detection Outside of Consciousness and Its Defensive Avoidance
Freud proposes that in unconscious processing, logical connections are also (heavily) based upon phonological similarities. Repressed concerns, for example, would also be expressed by way of phonologic ambiguity. In order to investigate a possible unconscious influence of phonological similarity, 31...
Autores principales: | , , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2019
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6460346/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31024274 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00077 |
_version_ | 1783410316883787776 |
---|---|
author | Bazan, Ariane Kushwaha, Ramesh Winer, E. Samuel Snodgrass, J. Michael Brakel, Linda A. W. Shevrin, Howard |
author_facet | Bazan, Ariane Kushwaha, Ramesh Winer, E. Samuel Snodgrass, J. Michael Brakel, Linda A. W. Shevrin, Howard |
author_sort | Bazan, Ariane |
collection | PubMed |
description | Freud proposes that in unconscious processing, logical connections are also (heavily) based upon phonological similarities. Repressed concerns, for example, would also be expressed by way of phonologic ambiguity. In order to investigate a possible unconscious influence of phonological similarity, 31 participants were submitted to a tachistoscopic subliminal priming experiment, with prime and target presented at 1 ms. In the experimental condition, the prime and one of the 2 targets were phonological reversed forms of each other, though graphemically dissimilar (e.g., “nice” and “sign”); in the control condition the targets were pseudo-randomly attributed to primes to which they don't belong. The experimental task was to “blindly” pick the choice most similar to the prime. ERPs were measured with a focus on the N320, which is known to react selectively to phonological mismatch in supraliminal visual word presentations. The N320 amplitude-effects at the electrodes on the midline and at the left of the brain significantly predicted the participants' net behavioral choices more than half a second later, while their subjective experience is one of arbitrariness. Moreover, the social desirability score (SDS) significantly correlates with both the behavioral and the N320 brain responses of the participants. It is proposed that in participants with low SDS the phonological target induces an expected reduction of N320 and this increases their probability to pick this target. In contrast, high defensive participants have a perplexed brain reaction upon the phonological target, with a negatively peaking N320 as compared to control and this leads them to avoid this target more often. Social desirability, which is understood as reflecting defensiveness, might also manifest itself as a defense against the (energy-consuming) ambiguity of language. The specificity of this study is that all of this is happening totally out of awareness and at the level of very elementary linguistic distinctions. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-6460346 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2019 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-64603462019-04-25 Phonological Ambiguity Detection Outside of Consciousness and Its Defensive Avoidance Bazan, Ariane Kushwaha, Ramesh Winer, E. Samuel Snodgrass, J. Michael Brakel, Linda A. W. Shevrin, Howard Front Hum Neurosci Neuroscience Freud proposes that in unconscious processing, logical connections are also (heavily) based upon phonological similarities. Repressed concerns, for example, would also be expressed by way of phonologic ambiguity. In order to investigate a possible unconscious influence of phonological similarity, 31 participants were submitted to a tachistoscopic subliminal priming experiment, with prime and target presented at 1 ms. In the experimental condition, the prime and one of the 2 targets were phonological reversed forms of each other, though graphemically dissimilar (e.g., “nice” and “sign”); in the control condition the targets were pseudo-randomly attributed to primes to which they don't belong. The experimental task was to “blindly” pick the choice most similar to the prime. ERPs were measured with a focus on the N320, which is known to react selectively to phonological mismatch in supraliminal visual word presentations. The N320 amplitude-effects at the electrodes on the midline and at the left of the brain significantly predicted the participants' net behavioral choices more than half a second later, while their subjective experience is one of arbitrariness. Moreover, the social desirability score (SDS) significantly correlates with both the behavioral and the N320 brain responses of the participants. It is proposed that in participants with low SDS the phonological target induces an expected reduction of N320 and this increases their probability to pick this target. In contrast, high defensive participants have a perplexed brain reaction upon the phonological target, with a negatively peaking N320 as compared to control and this leads them to avoid this target more often. Social desirability, which is understood as reflecting defensiveness, might also manifest itself as a defense against the (energy-consuming) ambiguity of language. The specificity of this study is that all of this is happening totally out of awareness and at the level of very elementary linguistic distinctions. Frontiers Media S.A. 2019-04-05 /pmc/articles/PMC6460346/ /pubmed/31024274 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00077 Text en Copyright © 2019 Bazan, Kushwaha, Winer, Snodgrass, Brakel and Shevrin. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Neuroscience Bazan, Ariane Kushwaha, Ramesh Winer, E. Samuel Snodgrass, J. Michael Brakel, Linda A. W. Shevrin, Howard Phonological Ambiguity Detection Outside of Consciousness and Its Defensive Avoidance |
title | Phonological Ambiguity Detection Outside of Consciousness and Its Defensive Avoidance |
title_full | Phonological Ambiguity Detection Outside of Consciousness and Its Defensive Avoidance |
title_fullStr | Phonological Ambiguity Detection Outside of Consciousness and Its Defensive Avoidance |
title_full_unstemmed | Phonological Ambiguity Detection Outside of Consciousness and Its Defensive Avoidance |
title_short | Phonological Ambiguity Detection Outside of Consciousness and Its Defensive Avoidance |
title_sort | phonological ambiguity detection outside of consciousness and its defensive avoidance |
topic | Neuroscience |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6460346/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31024274 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00077 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT bazanariane phonologicalambiguitydetectionoutsideofconsciousnessanditsdefensiveavoidance AT kushwaharamesh phonologicalambiguitydetectionoutsideofconsciousnessanditsdefensiveavoidance AT wineresamuel phonologicalambiguitydetectionoutsideofconsciousnessanditsdefensiveavoidance AT snodgrassjmichael phonologicalambiguitydetectionoutsideofconsciousnessanditsdefensiveavoidance AT brakellindaaw phonologicalambiguitydetectionoutsideofconsciousnessanditsdefensiveavoidance AT shevrinhoward phonologicalambiguitydetectionoutsideofconsciousnessanditsdefensiveavoidance |