Cargando…
Cognitive Performance among Cognitively Healthy Adults Aged 30–100 Years
BACKGROUND/AIMS: To detect cognitive decline in older adults, measures of verbal fluency and verbal memory are widely used. Less is known about performance in these measures in younger persons or according to education level and gender. We investigated cognitive performance according to age, educati...
Autores principales: | , , , , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
S. Karger AG
2019
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6477494/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31043960 http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000495657 |
_version_ | 1783413028065116160 |
---|---|
author | Alenius, Minna Koskinen, Sanna Hallikainen, Ilona Ngandu, Tiia Lipsanen, Jari Sainio, Päivi Tuulio-Henriksson, Annamari Hänninen, Tuomo |
author_facet | Alenius, Minna Koskinen, Sanna Hallikainen, Ilona Ngandu, Tiia Lipsanen, Jari Sainio, Päivi Tuulio-Henriksson, Annamari Hänninen, Tuomo |
author_sort | Alenius, Minna |
collection | PubMed |
description | BACKGROUND/AIMS: To detect cognitive decline in older adults, measures of verbal fluency and verbal memory are widely used. Less is known about performance in these measures in younger persons or according to education level and gender. We investigated cognitive performance according to age, education and gender among cognitively healthy adults aged 30–100 years. METHODS: The study population comprised 4,174 cognitively healthy persons participating in the nationally representative Finnish Health 2011 survey. Cognitive assessment included verbal fluency, word list memory, word list recall and word list savings from the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease neuropsychological battery. RESULTS: Total variance in the cognitive test performance explained by age, education and gender varied from 12.3 to 31.2%. A decreasing trend in cognitive performance existed in all subtests by advancing age, with differences appearing between 50 and 55 years. Persons with the highest-education level performed best for all measures. For the participants < 55 years, education explained part of the variance, while age and gender did not. CONCLUSIONS: When assessing cognition, age and education should be accounted for in more detail in research and clinical practice. Additionally, the cohort effect and its potential impact on the renewal cycle of future normative values for cognitive tests should be considered. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-6477494 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2019 |
publisher | S. Karger AG |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-64774942019-05-01 Cognitive Performance among Cognitively Healthy Adults Aged 30–100 Years Alenius, Minna Koskinen, Sanna Hallikainen, Ilona Ngandu, Tiia Lipsanen, Jari Sainio, Päivi Tuulio-Henriksson, Annamari Hänninen, Tuomo Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra Original Research Article BACKGROUND/AIMS: To detect cognitive decline in older adults, measures of verbal fluency and verbal memory are widely used. Less is known about performance in these measures in younger persons or according to education level and gender. We investigated cognitive performance according to age, education and gender among cognitively healthy adults aged 30–100 years. METHODS: The study population comprised 4,174 cognitively healthy persons participating in the nationally representative Finnish Health 2011 survey. Cognitive assessment included verbal fluency, word list memory, word list recall and word list savings from the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease neuropsychological battery. RESULTS: Total variance in the cognitive test performance explained by age, education and gender varied from 12.3 to 31.2%. A decreasing trend in cognitive performance existed in all subtests by advancing age, with differences appearing between 50 and 55 years. Persons with the highest-education level performed best for all measures. For the participants < 55 years, education explained part of the variance, while age and gender did not. CONCLUSIONS: When assessing cognition, age and education should be accounted for in more detail in research and clinical practice. Additionally, the cohort effect and its potential impact on the renewal cycle of future normative values for cognitive tests should be considered. S. Karger AG 2019-02-01 /pmc/articles/PMC6477494/ /pubmed/31043960 http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000495657 Text en Copyright © 2019 by S. Karger AG, Basel http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ This article is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License (CC BY-NC-ND) (http://www.karger.com/Services/OpenAccessLicense). Usage and distribution for commercial purposes as well as any distribution of modified material requires written permission. |
spellingShingle | Original Research Article Alenius, Minna Koskinen, Sanna Hallikainen, Ilona Ngandu, Tiia Lipsanen, Jari Sainio, Päivi Tuulio-Henriksson, Annamari Hänninen, Tuomo Cognitive Performance among Cognitively Healthy Adults Aged 30–100 Years |
title | Cognitive Performance among Cognitively Healthy Adults Aged 30–100 Years |
title_full | Cognitive Performance among Cognitively Healthy Adults Aged 30–100 Years |
title_fullStr | Cognitive Performance among Cognitively Healthy Adults Aged 30–100 Years |
title_full_unstemmed | Cognitive Performance among Cognitively Healthy Adults Aged 30–100 Years |
title_short | Cognitive Performance among Cognitively Healthy Adults Aged 30–100 Years |
title_sort | cognitive performance among cognitively healthy adults aged 30–100 years |
topic | Original Research Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6477494/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31043960 http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000495657 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT aleniusminna cognitiveperformanceamongcognitivelyhealthyadultsaged30100years AT koskinensanna cognitiveperformanceamongcognitivelyhealthyadultsaged30100years AT hallikainenilona cognitiveperformanceamongcognitivelyhealthyadultsaged30100years AT ngandutiia cognitiveperformanceamongcognitivelyhealthyadultsaged30100years AT lipsanenjari cognitiveperformanceamongcognitivelyhealthyadultsaged30100years AT sainiopaivi cognitiveperformanceamongcognitivelyhealthyadultsaged30100years AT tuuliohenrikssonannamari cognitiveperformanceamongcognitivelyhealthyadultsaged30100years AT hanninentuomo cognitiveperformanceamongcognitivelyhealthyadultsaged30100years |