Cargando…

Eye-specific pattern-motion signals support the perception of three-dimensional motion

An object moving through three-dimensional (3D) space typically yields different patterns of velocities in each eye. For an interocular velocity difference cue to be used, some instances of real 3D motion in the environment (e.g., when a moving object is partially occluded) would require an interocu...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Joo, Sung Jun, Greer, Devon A., Cormack, Lawrence K., Huk, Alexander C.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 2019
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6482860/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31013523
http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/19.4.27
_version_ 1783413960631910400
author Joo, Sung Jun
Greer, Devon A.
Cormack, Lawrence K.
Huk, Alexander C.
author_facet Joo, Sung Jun
Greer, Devon A.
Cormack, Lawrence K.
Huk, Alexander C.
author_sort Joo, Sung Jun
collection PubMed
description An object moving through three-dimensional (3D) space typically yields different patterns of velocities in each eye. For an interocular velocity difference cue to be used, some instances of real 3D motion in the environment (e.g., when a moving object is partially occluded) would require an interocular velocity difference computation that operates on motion signals that are not only monocular (or eye specific) but also depend on each eye's two-dimensional (2D) direction being estimated over regions larger than the size of V1 receptive fields (i.e., global pattern motion). We investigated this possibility using 3D motion aftereffects (MAEs) with stimuli comprising many small, drifting Gabor elements. Conventional frontoparallel (2D) MAEs were local—highly sensitive to the test elements being in the same locations as the adaptor (Experiment 1). In contrast, 3D MAEs were robust to the test elements being in different retinal locations than the adaptor, indicating that 3D motion processing involves relatively global spatial pooling of motion signals (Experiment 2). The 3D MAEs were strong even when the local elements were in unmatched locations across the two eyes during adaptation, as well as when the adapting stimulus elements were randomly oriented, and specified global motion via the intersection of constraints (Experiment 3). These results bolster the notion of eye-specific computation of 2D pattern motion (involving global pooling of local, eye-specific motion signals) for the purpose of computing 3D motion, and highlight the idea that classically “late” computations such as pattern motion can be done in a manner that retains information about the eye of origin.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-6482860
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2019
publisher The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-64828602019-05-07 Eye-specific pattern-motion signals support the perception of three-dimensional motion Joo, Sung Jun Greer, Devon A. Cormack, Lawrence K. Huk, Alexander C. J Vis Article An object moving through three-dimensional (3D) space typically yields different patterns of velocities in each eye. For an interocular velocity difference cue to be used, some instances of real 3D motion in the environment (e.g., when a moving object is partially occluded) would require an interocular velocity difference computation that operates on motion signals that are not only monocular (or eye specific) but also depend on each eye's two-dimensional (2D) direction being estimated over regions larger than the size of V1 receptive fields (i.e., global pattern motion). We investigated this possibility using 3D motion aftereffects (MAEs) with stimuli comprising many small, drifting Gabor elements. Conventional frontoparallel (2D) MAEs were local—highly sensitive to the test elements being in the same locations as the adaptor (Experiment 1). In contrast, 3D MAEs were robust to the test elements being in different retinal locations than the adaptor, indicating that 3D motion processing involves relatively global spatial pooling of motion signals (Experiment 2). The 3D MAEs were strong even when the local elements were in unmatched locations across the two eyes during adaptation, as well as when the adapting stimulus elements were randomly oriented, and specified global motion via the intersection of constraints (Experiment 3). These results bolster the notion of eye-specific computation of 2D pattern motion (involving global pooling of local, eye-specific motion signals) for the purpose of computing 3D motion, and highlight the idea that classically “late” computations such as pattern motion can be done in a manner that retains information about the eye of origin. The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 2019-04-23 /pmc/articles/PMC6482860/ /pubmed/31013523 http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/19.4.27 Text en Copyright 2019 The Authors http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
spellingShingle Article
Joo, Sung Jun
Greer, Devon A.
Cormack, Lawrence K.
Huk, Alexander C.
Eye-specific pattern-motion signals support the perception of three-dimensional motion
title Eye-specific pattern-motion signals support the perception of three-dimensional motion
title_full Eye-specific pattern-motion signals support the perception of three-dimensional motion
title_fullStr Eye-specific pattern-motion signals support the perception of three-dimensional motion
title_full_unstemmed Eye-specific pattern-motion signals support the perception of three-dimensional motion
title_short Eye-specific pattern-motion signals support the perception of three-dimensional motion
title_sort eye-specific pattern-motion signals support the perception of three-dimensional motion
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6482860/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31013523
http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/19.4.27
work_keys_str_mv AT joosungjun eyespecificpatternmotionsignalssupporttheperceptionofthreedimensionalmotion
AT greerdevona eyespecificpatternmotionsignalssupporttheperceptionofthreedimensionalmotion
AT cormacklawrencek eyespecificpatternmotionsignalssupporttheperceptionofthreedimensionalmotion
AT hukalexanderc eyespecificpatternmotionsignalssupporttheperceptionofthreedimensionalmotion