Cargando…

Word position coding in reading is noisy

In the present article, we investigate a largely unstudied cognitive process: word position coding. The question of how readers perceive word order is not trivial: Recent research has suggested that readers associate activated word representations with plausible locations in a sentence-level represe...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Snell, Joshua, Grainger, Jonathan
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Springer US 2019
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6488547/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30798470
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01574-0
_version_ 1783414657830092800
author Snell, Joshua
Grainger, Jonathan
author_facet Snell, Joshua
Grainger, Jonathan
author_sort Snell, Joshua
collection PubMed
description In the present article, we investigate a largely unstudied cognitive process: word position coding. The question of how readers perceive word order is not trivial: Recent research has suggested that readers associate activated word representations with plausible locations in a sentence-level representation. Rather than simply being dictated by the order in which words are recognized, word position coding may be influenced by bottom-up visual cues (e.g., word length information), as well as by top-down expectations. Here we assessed how flexible word position coding is. We let readers make grammaticality judgments about four-word sentences. The incorrect sentences were constructed by transposing two words in a correct sentence (e.g., “the man can run” became “the can man run”). The critical comparison was between two types of incorrect sentence: one with a transposition of the inner two words, and one with a transposition of the outer two words (“run man can the”). We reasoned that under limited (local) flexibility, it should be easier to classify the outer-transposed sentences as incorrect, because the words were farther away from their plausible locations in this condition. If words were recognized irrespective of location, on the other hand, there should be no difference between these two conditions. As it turned out, we observed longer response times and higher error rates for inner- than for outer-transposed sentences, indicating that local flexibility and top-down expectations can jointly lead the reader to confuse the locations of words, with a probability that increases as the distance between the plausible and actual locations of a word decreases. We conclude that word position coding is subject to a moderate amount of noise.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-6488547
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2019
publisher Springer US
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-64885472019-05-17 Word position coding in reading is noisy Snell, Joshua Grainger, Jonathan Psychon Bull Rev Article In the present article, we investigate a largely unstudied cognitive process: word position coding. The question of how readers perceive word order is not trivial: Recent research has suggested that readers associate activated word representations with plausible locations in a sentence-level representation. Rather than simply being dictated by the order in which words are recognized, word position coding may be influenced by bottom-up visual cues (e.g., word length information), as well as by top-down expectations. Here we assessed how flexible word position coding is. We let readers make grammaticality judgments about four-word sentences. The incorrect sentences were constructed by transposing two words in a correct sentence (e.g., “the man can run” became “the can man run”). The critical comparison was between two types of incorrect sentence: one with a transposition of the inner two words, and one with a transposition of the outer two words (“run man can the”). We reasoned that under limited (local) flexibility, it should be easier to classify the outer-transposed sentences as incorrect, because the words were farther away from their plausible locations in this condition. If words were recognized irrespective of location, on the other hand, there should be no difference between these two conditions. As it turned out, we observed longer response times and higher error rates for inner- than for outer-transposed sentences, indicating that local flexibility and top-down expectations can jointly lead the reader to confuse the locations of words, with a probability that increases as the distance between the plausible and actual locations of a word decreases. We conclude that word position coding is subject to a moderate amount of noise. Springer US 2019-02-23 2019 /pmc/articles/PMC6488547/ /pubmed/30798470 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01574-0 Text en © The Author(s) 2019 OpenAccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
spellingShingle Article
Snell, Joshua
Grainger, Jonathan
Word position coding in reading is noisy
title Word position coding in reading is noisy
title_full Word position coding in reading is noisy
title_fullStr Word position coding in reading is noisy
title_full_unstemmed Word position coding in reading is noisy
title_short Word position coding in reading is noisy
title_sort word position coding in reading is noisy
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6488547/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30798470
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01574-0
work_keys_str_mv AT snelljoshua wordpositioncodinginreadingisnoisy
AT graingerjonathan wordpositioncodinginreadingisnoisy