Cargando…

How Do Addressees Exploit Conventionalizations? From a Negative Reference to an ad hoc Implicature

A negative reference, such as “not the sculpture” (where the sculpture is a name the speaker had only just invented to describe an unconventional-looking object and where the negation is saying that she does not currently desire that object), seems like a perilous and linguistically underdetermined...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Kronmüller, Edmundo, Noveck, Ira
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Frontiers Media S.A. 2019
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6618899/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31333532
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01461
_version_ 1783433903818670080
author Kronmüller, Edmundo
Noveck, Ira
author_facet Kronmüller, Edmundo
Noveck, Ira
author_sort Kronmüller, Edmundo
collection PubMed
description A negative reference, such as “not the sculpture” (where the sculpture is a name the speaker had only just invented to describe an unconventional-looking object and where the negation is saying that she does not currently desire that object), seems like a perilous and linguistically underdetermined way to point to another object, especially when there are three objects to choose from. To succeed, it obliges listeners to rely on contextual elements to determine which object the speaker has in mind. Prior work has shown that pragmatic inference-making plays a crucial role in such an interpretation process. When a negative reference leaves two candidate objects to choose from, listeners avoid an object that had been previously named, preferring instead an unconventional-looking object that had remained unnamed (Kronmüller et al., 2017). In the present study, we build over these findings by maintaining our focus on the two remaining objects (what we call the second and third objects) as we systematically vary two features. With respect to the second object – which is always unconventional looking – we vary whether or not it has been given a name. With respect to the third object – which is never named – we vary whether it is unconventional or conventional looking (for the latter, imagine an object that clearly resembles a bicycle). As revealed by selection patterns and eye-movements in a visual-world eye-tracking paradigm, we replicate our previous findings that show that participants choose randomly when both of the remaining objects are unconventional looking and unnamed and that they opt reliably in favor of the most nondescript (the unnamed unconventional looking) object when the second object is named. We show further that (unnamed) conventional-looking objects provide similar outcomes when juxtaposed with an unnamed unconventional object (participants prefer the most non-descript as opposed to the conventional-looking object). Nevertheless, effects emerging from the conventional (unnamed) case are not as strong as those found with respect to those reported when an unconventional object is named. In describing participants’ choices in the non-random cases, we propose that addressees rely on the construction of an ad hoc implicature that takes into account which object can be eliminated from consideration, given that the speaker did not explicitly name it.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-6618899
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2019
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-66188992019-07-22 How Do Addressees Exploit Conventionalizations? From a Negative Reference to an ad hoc Implicature Kronmüller, Edmundo Noveck, Ira Front Psychol Psychology A negative reference, such as “not the sculpture” (where the sculpture is a name the speaker had only just invented to describe an unconventional-looking object and where the negation is saying that she does not currently desire that object), seems like a perilous and linguistically underdetermined way to point to another object, especially when there are three objects to choose from. To succeed, it obliges listeners to rely on contextual elements to determine which object the speaker has in mind. Prior work has shown that pragmatic inference-making plays a crucial role in such an interpretation process. When a negative reference leaves two candidate objects to choose from, listeners avoid an object that had been previously named, preferring instead an unconventional-looking object that had remained unnamed (Kronmüller et al., 2017). In the present study, we build over these findings by maintaining our focus on the two remaining objects (what we call the second and third objects) as we systematically vary two features. With respect to the second object – which is always unconventional looking – we vary whether or not it has been given a name. With respect to the third object – which is never named – we vary whether it is unconventional or conventional looking (for the latter, imagine an object that clearly resembles a bicycle). As revealed by selection patterns and eye-movements in a visual-world eye-tracking paradigm, we replicate our previous findings that show that participants choose randomly when both of the remaining objects are unconventional looking and unnamed and that they opt reliably in favor of the most nondescript (the unnamed unconventional looking) object when the second object is named. We show further that (unnamed) conventional-looking objects provide similar outcomes when juxtaposed with an unnamed unconventional object (participants prefer the most non-descript as opposed to the conventional-looking object). Nevertheless, effects emerging from the conventional (unnamed) case are not as strong as those found with respect to those reported when an unconventional object is named. In describing participants’ choices in the non-random cases, we propose that addressees rely on the construction of an ad hoc implicature that takes into account which object can be eliminated from consideration, given that the speaker did not explicitly name it. Frontiers Media S.A. 2019-07-03 /pmc/articles/PMC6618899/ /pubmed/31333532 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01461 Text en Copyright © 2019 Kronmüller and Noveck. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
spellingShingle Psychology
Kronmüller, Edmundo
Noveck, Ira
How Do Addressees Exploit Conventionalizations? From a Negative Reference to an ad hoc Implicature
title How Do Addressees Exploit Conventionalizations? From a Negative Reference to an ad hoc Implicature
title_full How Do Addressees Exploit Conventionalizations? From a Negative Reference to an ad hoc Implicature
title_fullStr How Do Addressees Exploit Conventionalizations? From a Negative Reference to an ad hoc Implicature
title_full_unstemmed How Do Addressees Exploit Conventionalizations? From a Negative Reference to an ad hoc Implicature
title_short How Do Addressees Exploit Conventionalizations? From a Negative Reference to an ad hoc Implicature
title_sort how do addressees exploit conventionalizations? from a negative reference to an ad hoc implicature
topic Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6618899/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31333532
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01461
work_keys_str_mv AT kronmulleredmundo howdoaddresseesexploitconventionalizationsfromanegativereferencetoanadhocimplicature
AT noveckira howdoaddresseesexploitconventionalizationsfromanegativereferencetoanadhocimplicature