Cargando…
Familiarity with visual stimuli boosts recency bias in macaques
To probe how non-human primates (NHPs) decode temporal dynamic stimuli, we used a two-alternative forced choice task (2AFC), where the cue was dynamic: a movie snippet drawn from an animation that transforms one image into another. When the cue was drawn from either the beginning or end of the anima...
Autores principales: | , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
PeerJ Inc.
2019
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6882415/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31788361 http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.8105 |
_version_ | 1783474157654114304 |
---|---|
author | Brunet, Nicolas Jagadeesh, Bharathi |
author_facet | Brunet, Nicolas Jagadeesh, Bharathi |
author_sort | Brunet, Nicolas |
collection | PubMed |
description | To probe how non-human primates (NHPs) decode temporal dynamic stimuli, we used a two-alternative forced choice task (2AFC), where the cue was dynamic: a movie snippet drawn from an animation that transforms one image into another. When the cue was drawn from either the beginning or end of the animation, thus heavily weighted towards one (the target) of both images (the choice pair), then primates performed at high levels of accuracy. For a subset of trials, however, the cue was ambiguous, drawn from the middle of the animation, containing information that could be associated to either image. Those trials, rewarded randomly and independent of choice, offered an opportunity to study the strategy the animals used trying to decode the cue. Despite being ambiguous, the primates exhibited a clear strategy, suggesting they were not aware that reward was given non-differentially. More specifically, they relied more on information provided at the end than at the beginning of those cues, consistent with the recency effect reported by numerous serial position studies. Interestingly and counterintuitively, this effect became stronger for sessions where the primates were already familiar with the stimuli. In other words, despite having rehearsed with the same stimuli in a previous session, the animals relied even more on a decision strategy that did not yield any benefits during a previous session. In the discussion section we speculate on what might cause this behavioral shift towards stronger bias, as well as why this behavior shows similarities with a repetition bias in humans known as the illusory truth effect. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-6882415 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2019 |
publisher | PeerJ Inc. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-68824152019-11-29 Familiarity with visual stimuli boosts recency bias in macaques Brunet, Nicolas Jagadeesh, Bharathi PeerJ Animal Behavior To probe how non-human primates (NHPs) decode temporal dynamic stimuli, we used a two-alternative forced choice task (2AFC), where the cue was dynamic: a movie snippet drawn from an animation that transforms one image into another. When the cue was drawn from either the beginning or end of the animation, thus heavily weighted towards one (the target) of both images (the choice pair), then primates performed at high levels of accuracy. For a subset of trials, however, the cue was ambiguous, drawn from the middle of the animation, containing information that could be associated to either image. Those trials, rewarded randomly and independent of choice, offered an opportunity to study the strategy the animals used trying to decode the cue. Despite being ambiguous, the primates exhibited a clear strategy, suggesting they were not aware that reward was given non-differentially. More specifically, they relied more on information provided at the end than at the beginning of those cues, consistent with the recency effect reported by numerous serial position studies. Interestingly and counterintuitively, this effect became stronger for sessions where the primates were already familiar with the stimuli. In other words, despite having rehearsed with the same stimuli in a previous session, the animals relied even more on a decision strategy that did not yield any benefits during a previous session. In the discussion section we speculate on what might cause this behavioral shift towards stronger bias, as well as why this behavior shows similarities with a repetition bias in humans known as the illusory truth effect. PeerJ Inc. 2019-11-25 /pmc/articles/PMC6882415/ /pubmed/31788361 http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.8105 Text en ©2019 Brunet and Jagadeesh https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, reproduction and adaptation in any medium and for any purpose provided that it is properly attributed. For attribution, the original author(s), title, publication source (PeerJ) and either DOI or URL of the article must be cited. |
spellingShingle | Animal Behavior Brunet, Nicolas Jagadeesh, Bharathi Familiarity with visual stimuli boosts recency bias in macaques |
title | Familiarity with visual stimuli boosts recency bias in macaques |
title_full | Familiarity with visual stimuli boosts recency bias in macaques |
title_fullStr | Familiarity with visual stimuli boosts recency bias in macaques |
title_full_unstemmed | Familiarity with visual stimuli boosts recency bias in macaques |
title_short | Familiarity with visual stimuli boosts recency bias in macaques |
title_sort | familiarity with visual stimuli boosts recency bias in macaques |
topic | Animal Behavior |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6882415/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31788361 http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.8105 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT brunetnicolas familiaritywithvisualstimuliboostsrecencybiasinmacaques AT jagadeeshbharathi familiaritywithvisualstimuliboostsrecencybiasinmacaques |