Cargando…

Can rhythm-induced attention improve the perceptual representation?

Temporal attention can be entrained exogenously to rhythms. Indeed, faster and more accurate responses were previously found when the target appeared in-phase with a preceding rhythm in comparison to when it was out of phase. However, the nature of this rhythm-induced attentional effect is not well...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Elbaz, Asaf, Yeshurun, Yaffa
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Public Library of Science 2020
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7162507/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32298272
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0231200
_version_ 1783523046019039232
author Elbaz, Asaf
Yeshurun, Yaffa
author_facet Elbaz, Asaf
Yeshurun, Yaffa
author_sort Elbaz, Asaf
collection PubMed
description Temporal attention can be entrained exogenously to rhythms. Indeed, faster and more accurate responses were previously found when the target appeared in-phase with a preceding rhythm in comparison to when it was out of phase. However, the nature of this rhythm-induced attentional effect is not well understood. To better understand the processes underlying rhythm-induced attention, we employed a continuous measure of perceived orientation and a mixture-model analysis. A trial in our study started with a sequence of auditory beeps separated by a fixed inter-beeps interval in the regular (rhythmic) condition or by variable inter-beeps intervals in the irregular condition. A visual target–a line embedded in a circle–followed the sequence. The ‘critical’ interval between the last beep and the target was chosen randomly from several possible Inter-Onset Intervals (IOIs), of which only one was in-phase with the rhythm. The target was followed by a probe line, and the participants were asked to rotate it to reproduce the target’s orientation. The measure of performance for a given trial was the difference in degrees between the orientation of the target and that reproduced by the observer. We found that guessing rate was lower with regular than irregular rhythms. However, there was no effect of rhythm type (regular vs irregular) on the quality of representation (measured as the variability in reproducing the target). Furthermore, the rhythm effect was present only when rhythm type was fixed within a block, and it was found with all IOIs, not just the in-phase IOI. This lack of specificity suggests that these results reflect a general effect of rhythm on alertness.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-7162507
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2020
publisher Public Library of Science
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-71625072020-04-21 Can rhythm-induced attention improve the perceptual representation? Elbaz, Asaf Yeshurun, Yaffa PLoS One Research Article Temporal attention can be entrained exogenously to rhythms. Indeed, faster and more accurate responses were previously found when the target appeared in-phase with a preceding rhythm in comparison to when it was out of phase. However, the nature of this rhythm-induced attentional effect is not well understood. To better understand the processes underlying rhythm-induced attention, we employed a continuous measure of perceived orientation and a mixture-model analysis. A trial in our study started with a sequence of auditory beeps separated by a fixed inter-beeps interval in the regular (rhythmic) condition or by variable inter-beeps intervals in the irregular condition. A visual target–a line embedded in a circle–followed the sequence. The ‘critical’ interval between the last beep and the target was chosen randomly from several possible Inter-Onset Intervals (IOIs), of which only one was in-phase with the rhythm. The target was followed by a probe line, and the participants were asked to rotate it to reproduce the target’s orientation. The measure of performance for a given trial was the difference in degrees between the orientation of the target and that reproduced by the observer. We found that guessing rate was lower with regular than irregular rhythms. However, there was no effect of rhythm type (regular vs irregular) on the quality of representation (measured as the variability in reproducing the target). Furthermore, the rhythm effect was present only when rhythm type was fixed within a block, and it was found with all IOIs, not just the in-phase IOI. This lack of specificity suggests that these results reflect a general effect of rhythm on alertness. Public Library of Science 2020-04-16 /pmc/articles/PMC7162507/ /pubmed/32298272 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0231200 Text en © 2020 Elbaz, Yeshurun http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
spellingShingle Research Article
Elbaz, Asaf
Yeshurun, Yaffa
Can rhythm-induced attention improve the perceptual representation?
title Can rhythm-induced attention improve the perceptual representation?
title_full Can rhythm-induced attention improve the perceptual representation?
title_fullStr Can rhythm-induced attention improve the perceptual representation?
title_full_unstemmed Can rhythm-induced attention improve the perceptual representation?
title_short Can rhythm-induced attention improve the perceptual representation?
title_sort can rhythm-induced attention improve the perceptual representation?
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7162507/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32298272
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0231200
work_keys_str_mv AT elbazasaf canrhythminducedattentionimprovetheperceptualrepresentation
AT yeshurunyaffa canrhythminducedattentionimprovetheperceptualrepresentation