Cargando…

Inattention and hyperactive/impulsive component scores do not differentiate between autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinical sample

BACKGROUND: Although there is high co-occurrence between ASD and ADHD, the nature of this co-occurrence remains unclear. Our study aimed to examine the underlying relationship between ASD and ADHD symptoms in a combined sample of children with a primary clinical diagnosis of ASD or ADHD. METHODS: Pa...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Krakowski, Aneta D., Cost, Katherine Tombeau, Anagnostou, Evdokia, Lai, Meng-Chuan, Crosbie, Jennifer, Schachar, Russell, Georgiades, Stelios, Duku, Eric, Szatmari, Peter
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: BioMed Central 2020
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7183643/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32334619
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s13229-020-00338-1
_version_ 1783526461183885312
author Krakowski, Aneta D.
Cost, Katherine Tombeau
Anagnostou, Evdokia
Lai, Meng-Chuan
Crosbie, Jennifer
Schachar, Russell
Georgiades, Stelios
Duku, Eric
Szatmari, Peter
author_facet Krakowski, Aneta D.
Cost, Katherine Tombeau
Anagnostou, Evdokia
Lai, Meng-Chuan
Crosbie, Jennifer
Schachar, Russell
Georgiades, Stelios
Duku, Eric
Szatmari, Peter
author_sort Krakowski, Aneta D.
collection PubMed
description BACKGROUND: Although there is high co-occurrence between ASD and ADHD, the nature of this co-occurrence remains unclear. Our study aimed to examine the underlying relationship between ASD and ADHD symptoms in a combined sample of children with a primary clinical diagnosis of ASD or ADHD. METHODS: Participants included children and youth (aged 3-20 years) with a clinical diagnosis of ASD (n = 303) or ADHD (n = 319) for a total of 622 participants. Parents of these children completed the social communication questionnaire (SCQ), a measure of autism symptoms, and the strengths and weaknesses of ADHD and normal behavior (SWAN) questionnaire, a measure of ADHD symptoms. A principal component analysis (PCA) was performed on combined SCQ and SWAN items, followed by a profile analysis comparing normalized component scores between diagnostic groups and gender. RESULTS: PCA revealed a four-component solution (inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, social-communication, and restricted, repetitive, behaviors, and interests (RRBI)), with no overlap between SCQ and SWAN items in the components. Children with ASD had higher component scores in social-communication and RRBI than children with ADHD, while there was no difference in inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive scores between diagnostic groups. Males had higher scores than females in social-communication, RRBI, and hyperactivity/impulsivity components in each diagnostic group. LIMITATIONS: We did not formally assess children with ASD for ADHD using our research-criteria for ADHD, and vice versa. High rates of co-occurring ADHD in ASD, for example, may have inflated component scores in inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity. A disadvantage with using single informant-based reports (i.e., parent-rated questionnaires) is that ASD and ADHD symptoms may be difficult to distinguish by parents, and may be interpreted differently between parents and clinicians. CONCLUSIONS: ASD and ADHD items loaded on separate components in our sample, suggesting that the measurement structure cannot explain the covariation between the two disorders in clinical samples. High levels of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity were seen in both ASD and ADHD in our clinical sample. This supports the need for a dimensional framework that examines neurodevelopmental domains across traditional diagnostic boundaries. Females also had lower component scores across social-communication, RRBI, and hyperactivity/impulsivity than males, suggesting that there may be gender-specific phenotypes related to the two conditions.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-7183643
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2020
publisher BioMed Central
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-71836432020-04-29 Inattention and hyperactive/impulsive component scores do not differentiate between autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinical sample Krakowski, Aneta D. Cost, Katherine Tombeau Anagnostou, Evdokia Lai, Meng-Chuan Crosbie, Jennifer Schachar, Russell Georgiades, Stelios Duku, Eric Szatmari, Peter Mol Autism Research BACKGROUND: Although there is high co-occurrence between ASD and ADHD, the nature of this co-occurrence remains unclear. Our study aimed to examine the underlying relationship between ASD and ADHD symptoms in a combined sample of children with a primary clinical diagnosis of ASD or ADHD. METHODS: Participants included children and youth (aged 3-20 years) with a clinical diagnosis of ASD (n = 303) or ADHD (n = 319) for a total of 622 participants. Parents of these children completed the social communication questionnaire (SCQ), a measure of autism symptoms, and the strengths and weaknesses of ADHD and normal behavior (SWAN) questionnaire, a measure of ADHD symptoms. A principal component analysis (PCA) was performed on combined SCQ and SWAN items, followed by a profile analysis comparing normalized component scores between diagnostic groups and gender. RESULTS: PCA revealed a four-component solution (inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, social-communication, and restricted, repetitive, behaviors, and interests (RRBI)), with no overlap between SCQ and SWAN items in the components. Children with ASD had higher component scores in social-communication and RRBI than children with ADHD, while there was no difference in inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive scores between diagnostic groups. Males had higher scores than females in social-communication, RRBI, and hyperactivity/impulsivity components in each diagnostic group. LIMITATIONS: We did not formally assess children with ASD for ADHD using our research-criteria for ADHD, and vice versa. High rates of co-occurring ADHD in ASD, for example, may have inflated component scores in inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity. A disadvantage with using single informant-based reports (i.e., parent-rated questionnaires) is that ASD and ADHD symptoms may be difficult to distinguish by parents, and may be interpreted differently between parents and clinicians. CONCLUSIONS: ASD and ADHD items loaded on separate components in our sample, suggesting that the measurement structure cannot explain the covariation between the two disorders in clinical samples. High levels of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity were seen in both ASD and ADHD in our clinical sample. This supports the need for a dimensional framework that examines neurodevelopmental domains across traditional diagnostic boundaries. Females also had lower component scores across social-communication, RRBI, and hyperactivity/impulsivity than males, suggesting that there may be gender-specific phenotypes related to the two conditions. BioMed Central 2020-04-25 /pmc/articles/PMC7183643/ /pubmed/32334619 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s13229-020-00338-1 Text en © The Author(s) 2020 Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
spellingShingle Research
Krakowski, Aneta D.
Cost, Katherine Tombeau
Anagnostou, Evdokia
Lai, Meng-Chuan
Crosbie, Jennifer
Schachar, Russell
Georgiades, Stelios
Duku, Eric
Szatmari, Peter
Inattention and hyperactive/impulsive component scores do not differentiate between autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinical sample
title Inattention and hyperactive/impulsive component scores do not differentiate between autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinical sample
title_full Inattention and hyperactive/impulsive component scores do not differentiate between autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinical sample
title_fullStr Inattention and hyperactive/impulsive component scores do not differentiate between autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinical sample
title_full_unstemmed Inattention and hyperactive/impulsive component scores do not differentiate between autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinical sample
title_short Inattention and hyperactive/impulsive component scores do not differentiate between autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinical sample
title_sort inattention and hyperactive/impulsive component scores do not differentiate between autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinical sample
topic Research
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7183643/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32334619
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s13229-020-00338-1
work_keys_str_mv AT krakowskianetad inattentionandhyperactiveimpulsivecomponentscoresdonotdifferentiatebetweenautismspectrumdisorderandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorderinaclinicalsample
AT costkatherinetombeau inattentionandhyperactiveimpulsivecomponentscoresdonotdifferentiatebetweenautismspectrumdisorderandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorderinaclinicalsample
AT anagnostouevdokia inattentionandhyperactiveimpulsivecomponentscoresdonotdifferentiatebetweenautismspectrumdisorderandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorderinaclinicalsample
AT laimengchuan inattentionandhyperactiveimpulsivecomponentscoresdonotdifferentiatebetweenautismspectrumdisorderandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorderinaclinicalsample
AT crosbiejennifer inattentionandhyperactiveimpulsivecomponentscoresdonotdifferentiatebetweenautismspectrumdisorderandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorderinaclinicalsample
AT schacharrussell inattentionandhyperactiveimpulsivecomponentscoresdonotdifferentiatebetweenautismspectrumdisorderandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorderinaclinicalsample
AT georgiadesstelios inattentionandhyperactiveimpulsivecomponentscoresdonotdifferentiatebetweenautismspectrumdisorderandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorderinaclinicalsample
AT dukueric inattentionandhyperactiveimpulsivecomponentscoresdonotdifferentiatebetweenautismspectrumdisorderandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorderinaclinicalsample
AT szatmaripeter inattentionandhyperactiveimpulsivecomponentscoresdonotdifferentiatebetweenautismspectrumdisorderandattentiondeficithyperactivitydisorderinaclinicalsample