Cargando…
Nectar mimicry: a new phenomenon
Nectar is the most common floral reward for flower-visiting flies, bees, bats and birds. Many flowers hide nectar in the floral tube and preclude sensing of nectar by flower-visitors from a distance. Even in those flowers that offer easily accessible nectar, the nectaries are mostly inconspicuous to...
Autores principales: | , , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Nature Publishing Group UK
2020
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7184725/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32341437 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-63997-3 |
_version_ | 1783526640841654272 |
---|---|
author | Lunau, Klaus Ren, Zong-Xin Fan, Xiao-Qing Trunschke, Judith Pyke, Graham H. Wang, Hong |
author_facet | Lunau, Klaus Ren, Zong-Xin Fan, Xiao-Qing Trunschke, Judith Pyke, Graham H. Wang, Hong |
author_sort | Lunau, Klaus |
collection | PubMed |
description | Nectar is the most common floral reward for flower-visiting flies, bees, bats and birds. Many flowers hide nectar in the floral tube and preclude sensing of nectar by flower-visitors from a distance. Even in those flowers that offer easily accessible nectar, the nectaries are mostly inconspicuous to the human eye and the amount of nectar is sparse. It is widely accepted that many flowers display nectar guides in order to direct flower-visitors towards the nectar. Using false colour photography, covering ultraviolet, blue and green ranges of wavelength, revealed a yet unknown conspicuousness of nectar, nectaries and false nectaries for bees due to concordant reflection in the ultraviolet range of wavelength. Nectars, many nectaries and false nectaries have glossy surfaces and reflect all incident light including UV-light. In most cases, this is not particularly conspicuous to the human eye, but highly visible for UV-sensitive insects, due to the fact that the glossy areas are often positioned in UV-absorbing central flower parts and thus produce a strong UV-signal. The optical contrast produced by the glossiness of small smooth areas in close proximity to nectar holders represents a widespread yet overlooked floral cue that nectarivorous flower-visitors might use to locate the floral nectar. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-7184725 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2020 |
publisher | Nature Publishing Group UK |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-71847252020-05-04 Nectar mimicry: a new phenomenon Lunau, Klaus Ren, Zong-Xin Fan, Xiao-Qing Trunschke, Judith Pyke, Graham H. Wang, Hong Sci Rep Article Nectar is the most common floral reward for flower-visiting flies, bees, bats and birds. Many flowers hide nectar in the floral tube and preclude sensing of nectar by flower-visitors from a distance. Even in those flowers that offer easily accessible nectar, the nectaries are mostly inconspicuous to the human eye and the amount of nectar is sparse. It is widely accepted that many flowers display nectar guides in order to direct flower-visitors towards the nectar. Using false colour photography, covering ultraviolet, blue and green ranges of wavelength, revealed a yet unknown conspicuousness of nectar, nectaries and false nectaries for bees due to concordant reflection in the ultraviolet range of wavelength. Nectars, many nectaries and false nectaries have glossy surfaces and reflect all incident light including UV-light. In most cases, this is not particularly conspicuous to the human eye, but highly visible for UV-sensitive insects, due to the fact that the glossy areas are often positioned in UV-absorbing central flower parts and thus produce a strong UV-signal. The optical contrast produced by the glossiness of small smooth areas in close proximity to nectar holders represents a widespread yet overlooked floral cue that nectarivorous flower-visitors might use to locate the floral nectar. Nature Publishing Group UK 2020-04-27 /pmc/articles/PMC7184725/ /pubmed/32341437 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-63997-3 Text en © The Author(s) 2020 Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. |
spellingShingle | Article Lunau, Klaus Ren, Zong-Xin Fan, Xiao-Qing Trunschke, Judith Pyke, Graham H. Wang, Hong Nectar mimicry: a new phenomenon |
title | Nectar mimicry: a new phenomenon |
title_full | Nectar mimicry: a new phenomenon |
title_fullStr | Nectar mimicry: a new phenomenon |
title_full_unstemmed | Nectar mimicry: a new phenomenon |
title_short | Nectar mimicry: a new phenomenon |
title_sort | nectar mimicry: a new phenomenon |
topic | Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7184725/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32341437 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-63997-3 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT lunauklaus nectarmimicryanewphenomenon AT renzongxin nectarmimicryanewphenomenon AT fanxiaoqing nectarmimicryanewphenomenon AT trunschkejudith nectarmimicryanewphenomenon AT pykegrahamh nectarmimicryanewphenomenon AT wanghong nectarmimicryanewphenomenon |