Cargando…

The Comparative Psychology of Intelligence: Some Thirty Years Later

After re-reading Macphail’s (1987) essay “The Comparative Psychology of Intelligence” with all the associated commentaries, I was struck by how contemporary many of the arguments and counter-arguments still appear. Of course, we now know much more about the abilities of many more species (including...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autor principal: Pepperberg, Irene M.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Frontiers Media S.A. 2020
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7248277/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32508723
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00973
_version_ 1783538335701008384
author Pepperberg, Irene M.
author_facet Pepperberg, Irene M.
author_sort Pepperberg, Irene M.
collection PubMed
description After re-reading Macphail’s (1987) essay “The Comparative Psychology of Intelligence” with all the associated commentaries, I was struck by how contemporary many of the arguments and counter-arguments still appear. Of course, we now know much more about the abilities of many more species (including their neurobiology) and fewer researchers currently favor explanations of behavior based solely on associative processes; however, the role of contextual variables in comparative psychology still remains cloudy. I discuss these issues briefly. Given my research interests involving the cognitive and communicative abilities of Grey parrots, the one aspect of the original article upon which I feel I can comment in depth involves Macphail’s claims about the importance of language—and specifically syntax—in problem-solving and thus in placing humans above all other creatures. Granted, no other species has (or in my opinion is likely ever to acquire) everything that goes into what is considered “human language.” Nevertheless, several other species have acquired symbolic representation, and considerable information now exists upon which to base an argument that such acquisition by itself enables more complex and “human-like” cognitive processes. Such processes may form the basis of the kind of intelligence that is measured—not surprisingly—with human-based tasks, including the use of such representations as a means to directly query non-human subjects in ways not unlike those used with young children.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-7248277
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2020
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-72482772020-06-05 The Comparative Psychology of Intelligence: Some Thirty Years Later Pepperberg, Irene M. Front Psychol Psychology After re-reading Macphail’s (1987) essay “The Comparative Psychology of Intelligence” with all the associated commentaries, I was struck by how contemporary many of the arguments and counter-arguments still appear. Of course, we now know much more about the abilities of many more species (including their neurobiology) and fewer researchers currently favor explanations of behavior based solely on associative processes; however, the role of contextual variables in comparative psychology still remains cloudy. I discuss these issues briefly. Given my research interests involving the cognitive and communicative abilities of Grey parrots, the one aspect of the original article upon which I feel I can comment in depth involves Macphail’s claims about the importance of language—and specifically syntax—in problem-solving and thus in placing humans above all other creatures. Granted, no other species has (or in my opinion is likely ever to acquire) everything that goes into what is considered “human language.” Nevertheless, several other species have acquired symbolic representation, and considerable information now exists upon which to base an argument that such acquisition by itself enables more complex and “human-like” cognitive processes. Such processes may form the basis of the kind of intelligence that is measured—not surprisingly—with human-based tasks, including the use of such representations as a means to directly query non-human subjects in ways not unlike those used with young children. Frontiers Media S.A. 2020-05-19 /pmc/articles/PMC7248277/ /pubmed/32508723 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00973 Text en Copyright © 2020 Pepperberg. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
spellingShingle Psychology
Pepperberg, Irene M.
The Comparative Psychology of Intelligence: Some Thirty Years Later
title The Comparative Psychology of Intelligence: Some Thirty Years Later
title_full The Comparative Psychology of Intelligence: Some Thirty Years Later
title_fullStr The Comparative Psychology of Intelligence: Some Thirty Years Later
title_full_unstemmed The Comparative Psychology of Intelligence: Some Thirty Years Later
title_short The Comparative Psychology of Intelligence: Some Thirty Years Later
title_sort comparative psychology of intelligence: some thirty years later
topic Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7248277/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32508723
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00973
work_keys_str_mv AT pepperbergirenem thecomparativepsychologyofintelligencesomethirtyyearslater
AT pepperbergirenem comparativepsychologyofintelligencesomethirtyyearslater