Cargando…
Interactive Alignment and Lexical Triggering of Code-Switching in Bilingual Dialogue
When bilingual speakers use two languages in the same utterance, this is called code-switching. Previous research indicates that bilinguals’ likelihood to code-switch is enhanced when the utterance to be produced (1) contains a word with a similar form across languages (lexical triggering) and (2) i...
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2020
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7387648/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32793070 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01747 |
_version_ | 1783564168159297536 |
---|---|
author | Kootstra, Gerrit Jan Dijkstra, Ton van Hell, Janet G. |
author_facet | Kootstra, Gerrit Jan Dijkstra, Ton van Hell, Janet G. |
author_sort | Kootstra, Gerrit Jan |
collection | PubMed |
description | When bilingual speakers use two languages in the same utterance, this is called code-switching. Previous research indicates that bilinguals’ likelihood to code-switch is enhanced when the utterance to be produced (1) contains a word with a similar form across languages (lexical triggering) and (2) is preceded by a code-switched utterance, for example from a dialogue partner (interactive alignment/priming of code-switching). Both factors have mostly been tested on corpus data and have not yet been studied in combination. In two experiments, we therefore investigated the combined effects of interactive alignment and lexical triggering on code-switching. In Experiment 1, Dutch-English bilinguals described pictures to each other in a dialogue game where a confederate’s code-switching was manipulated. The participants were free to use either Dutch, English, or a combination of Dutch and English in describing the pictures, so they could voluntarily code-switch or not. The pictures contained a cognate [e.g., roos (rose)], a false friend [e.g., rok (skirt, false friend with rock)], or a control word [e.g., jas (coat)]. Participants code-switched more often when the confederate had just code-switched (indicating interactive alignment). They also code-switched more often when cognates were involved, but only when the confederate had just code-switched. This indicates that lexical triggering is driven by interactive alignment. False friends did not enhance the likelihood of code-switching. Experiment 2 used a similar dialogue game with participants from the same population but focused specifically on how to account for interactive alignment of code-switching. Rather than aligning on their dialogue partner’s pragmatic act of code-switching, bilinguals aligned on the language activation from the utterance produced by their dialogue partner. All in all, the results show how co-activation of languages at multiple levels of processing together influence bilinguals’ tendency to code-switch. The findings call for a perspective on bilingual language production in which cross-speaker and cross-language processes are combined. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-7387648 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2020 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-73876482020-08-12 Interactive Alignment and Lexical Triggering of Code-Switching in Bilingual Dialogue Kootstra, Gerrit Jan Dijkstra, Ton van Hell, Janet G. Front Psychol Psychology When bilingual speakers use two languages in the same utterance, this is called code-switching. Previous research indicates that bilinguals’ likelihood to code-switch is enhanced when the utterance to be produced (1) contains a word with a similar form across languages (lexical triggering) and (2) is preceded by a code-switched utterance, for example from a dialogue partner (interactive alignment/priming of code-switching). Both factors have mostly been tested on corpus data and have not yet been studied in combination. In two experiments, we therefore investigated the combined effects of interactive alignment and lexical triggering on code-switching. In Experiment 1, Dutch-English bilinguals described pictures to each other in a dialogue game where a confederate’s code-switching was manipulated. The participants were free to use either Dutch, English, or a combination of Dutch and English in describing the pictures, so they could voluntarily code-switch or not. The pictures contained a cognate [e.g., roos (rose)], a false friend [e.g., rok (skirt, false friend with rock)], or a control word [e.g., jas (coat)]. Participants code-switched more often when the confederate had just code-switched (indicating interactive alignment). They also code-switched more often when cognates were involved, but only when the confederate had just code-switched. This indicates that lexical triggering is driven by interactive alignment. False friends did not enhance the likelihood of code-switching. Experiment 2 used a similar dialogue game with participants from the same population but focused specifically on how to account for interactive alignment of code-switching. Rather than aligning on their dialogue partner’s pragmatic act of code-switching, bilinguals aligned on the language activation from the utterance produced by their dialogue partner. All in all, the results show how co-activation of languages at multiple levels of processing together influence bilinguals’ tendency to code-switch. The findings call for a perspective on bilingual language production in which cross-speaker and cross-language processes are combined. Frontiers Media S.A. 2020-07-22 /pmc/articles/PMC7387648/ /pubmed/32793070 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01747 Text en Copyright © 2020 Kootstra, Dijkstra and van Hell. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Psychology Kootstra, Gerrit Jan Dijkstra, Ton van Hell, Janet G. Interactive Alignment and Lexical Triggering of Code-Switching in Bilingual Dialogue |
title | Interactive Alignment and Lexical Triggering of Code-Switching in Bilingual Dialogue |
title_full | Interactive Alignment and Lexical Triggering of Code-Switching in Bilingual Dialogue |
title_fullStr | Interactive Alignment and Lexical Triggering of Code-Switching in Bilingual Dialogue |
title_full_unstemmed | Interactive Alignment and Lexical Triggering of Code-Switching in Bilingual Dialogue |
title_short | Interactive Alignment and Lexical Triggering of Code-Switching in Bilingual Dialogue |
title_sort | interactive alignment and lexical triggering of code-switching in bilingual dialogue |
topic | Psychology |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7387648/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32793070 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01747 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT kootstragerritjan interactivealignmentandlexicaltriggeringofcodeswitchinginbilingualdialogue AT dijkstraton interactivealignmentandlexicaltriggeringofcodeswitchinginbilingualdialogue AT vanhelljanetg interactivealignmentandlexicaltriggeringofcodeswitchinginbilingualdialogue |