Cargando…

Different luminance- and texture-defined contrast sensitivity profiles for school-aged children

Our current understanding of how the visual brain develops is based largely on the study of luminance-defined information processing. This approach, however, is somewhat limiting, since everyday scenes are composed of complex images, consisting of information characterized by physical attributes rel...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Silvestre, Daphné, Guy, Jacalyn, Hanck, Julie, Cornish, Kim, Bertone, Armando
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Nature Publishing Group UK 2020
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7400652/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32747677
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-69802-5
_version_ 1783566413183582208
author Silvestre, Daphné
Guy, Jacalyn
Hanck, Julie
Cornish, Kim
Bertone, Armando
author_facet Silvestre, Daphné
Guy, Jacalyn
Hanck, Julie
Cornish, Kim
Bertone, Armando
author_sort Silvestre, Daphné
collection PubMed
description Our current understanding of how the visual brain develops is based largely on the study of luminance-defined information processing. This approach, however, is somewhat limiting, since everyday scenes are composed of complex images, consisting of information characterized by physical attributes relating to both luminance and texture. Few studies have explored how contrast sensitivity to texture-defined information develops, particularly throughout the school-aged years. The current study investigated how contrast sensitivity to luminance- (luminance-modulated noise) and texture-defined (contrast-modulated noise) static gratings develops in school-aged children. Contrast sensitivity functions identified distinct profiles for luminance- and texture-defined gratings across spatial frequencies (SFs) and age. Sensitivity to luminance-defined gratings reached maturity in childhood by the ages of 9–10 years for all SFs (0.5, 1, 2, 4 and 8 cycles/degree or cpd). Sensitivity to texture-defined gratings reached maturity at 5–6 years for low SFs and 7–8 years for high SFs (i.e., 4 cpd). These results establish that the processing of luminance- and texture-defined information develop differently as a function of SF and age.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-7400652
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2020
publisher Nature Publishing Group UK
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-74006522020-08-04 Different luminance- and texture-defined contrast sensitivity profiles for school-aged children Silvestre, Daphné Guy, Jacalyn Hanck, Julie Cornish, Kim Bertone, Armando Sci Rep Article Our current understanding of how the visual brain develops is based largely on the study of luminance-defined information processing. This approach, however, is somewhat limiting, since everyday scenes are composed of complex images, consisting of information characterized by physical attributes relating to both luminance and texture. Few studies have explored how contrast sensitivity to texture-defined information develops, particularly throughout the school-aged years. The current study investigated how contrast sensitivity to luminance- (luminance-modulated noise) and texture-defined (contrast-modulated noise) static gratings develops in school-aged children. Contrast sensitivity functions identified distinct profiles for luminance- and texture-defined gratings across spatial frequencies (SFs) and age. Sensitivity to luminance-defined gratings reached maturity in childhood by the ages of 9–10 years for all SFs (0.5, 1, 2, 4 and 8 cycles/degree or cpd). Sensitivity to texture-defined gratings reached maturity at 5–6 years for low SFs and 7–8 years for high SFs (i.e., 4 cpd). These results establish that the processing of luminance- and texture-defined information develop differently as a function of SF and age. Nature Publishing Group UK 2020-08-03 /pmc/articles/PMC7400652/ /pubmed/32747677 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-69802-5 Text en © The Author(s) 2020 Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
spellingShingle Article
Silvestre, Daphné
Guy, Jacalyn
Hanck, Julie
Cornish, Kim
Bertone, Armando
Different luminance- and texture-defined contrast sensitivity profiles for school-aged children
title Different luminance- and texture-defined contrast sensitivity profiles for school-aged children
title_full Different luminance- and texture-defined contrast sensitivity profiles for school-aged children
title_fullStr Different luminance- and texture-defined contrast sensitivity profiles for school-aged children
title_full_unstemmed Different luminance- and texture-defined contrast sensitivity profiles for school-aged children
title_short Different luminance- and texture-defined contrast sensitivity profiles for school-aged children
title_sort different luminance- and texture-defined contrast sensitivity profiles for school-aged children
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7400652/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32747677
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-69802-5
work_keys_str_mv AT silvestredaphne differentluminanceandtexturedefinedcontrastsensitivityprofilesforschoolagedchildren
AT guyjacalyn differentluminanceandtexturedefinedcontrastsensitivityprofilesforschoolagedchildren
AT hanckjulie differentluminanceandtexturedefinedcontrastsensitivityprofilesforschoolagedchildren
AT cornishkim differentluminanceandtexturedefinedcontrastsensitivityprofilesforschoolagedchildren
AT bertonearmando differentluminanceandtexturedefinedcontrastsensitivityprofilesforschoolagedchildren