Cargando…

Chromatic fading following complete adaptation to unique hues

Profound vision loss occurs after prolonged exposure to an unchanging featureless visual environment. The effect is sometimes called visual fade. Here we investigate this phenomenon in the color domain using two different experiments. In the first experiment we determine the time needed for a colore...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Stanikunas, Rytis, Kulbokaite, Vaiva, Svegzda, Algimantas, Vaitkevicius, Henrikas, Daugirdiene, Ausra, Kulikowski, Janus J., Murray, Ian J.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 2020
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7416897/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32589196
http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.6.20
_version_ 1783569381685460992
author Stanikunas, Rytis
Kulbokaite, Vaiva
Svegzda, Algimantas
Vaitkevicius, Henrikas
Daugirdiene, Ausra
Kulikowski, Janus J.
Murray, Ian J.
author_facet Stanikunas, Rytis
Kulbokaite, Vaiva
Svegzda, Algimantas
Vaitkevicius, Henrikas
Daugirdiene, Ausra
Kulikowski, Janus J.
Murray, Ian J.
author_sort Stanikunas, Rytis
collection PubMed
description Profound vision loss occurs after prolonged exposure to an unchanging featureless visual environment. The effect is sometimes called visual fade. Here we investigate this phenomenon in the color domain using two different experiments. In the first experiment we determine the time needed for a colored background to appear achromatic. Four backgrounds were tested. Each represented the observers’ four unique hues. This adaptation time was compared with time to recover after adaptation Hue shifts at the end of the adaptation period were also measured. There were wide individual differences in adaptation times and recovery times. Overall recovery was faster than adaptation (p < 0.02). There were minimal shifts in hue. In the second experiment the changes in saturation (Munsell chroma) and lightness (Munsell value) of the background were monitored at six time intervals during the adapting process. Again asymmetric matching with Munsell samples was used. There were two distinct components to both the adaptation and recovery phases; one fast with time constant <1s, the other slow with time constant between 40 and 160s. The experiments show that the special case of visual fade involving color represents the sensory basis for many color-related effects involving adaptation.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-7416897
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2020
publisher The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-74168972020-08-24 Chromatic fading following complete adaptation to unique hues Stanikunas, Rytis Kulbokaite, Vaiva Svegzda, Algimantas Vaitkevicius, Henrikas Daugirdiene, Ausra Kulikowski, Janus J. Murray, Ian J. J Vis Article Profound vision loss occurs after prolonged exposure to an unchanging featureless visual environment. The effect is sometimes called visual fade. Here we investigate this phenomenon in the color domain using two different experiments. In the first experiment we determine the time needed for a colored background to appear achromatic. Four backgrounds were tested. Each represented the observers’ four unique hues. This adaptation time was compared with time to recover after adaptation Hue shifts at the end of the adaptation period were also measured. There were wide individual differences in adaptation times and recovery times. Overall recovery was faster than adaptation (p < 0.02). There were minimal shifts in hue. In the second experiment the changes in saturation (Munsell chroma) and lightness (Munsell value) of the background were monitored at six time intervals during the adapting process. Again asymmetric matching with Munsell samples was used. There were two distinct components to both the adaptation and recovery phases; one fast with time constant <1s, the other slow with time constant between 40 and 160s. The experiments show that the special case of visual fade involving color represents the sensory basis for many color-related effects involving adaptation. The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 2020-06-26 /pmc/articles/PMC7416897/ /pubmed/32589196 http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.6.20 Text en Copyright 2020 The Authors http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
spellingShingle Article
Stanikunas, Rytis
Kulbokaite, Vaiva
Svegzda, Algimantas
Vaitkevicius, Henrikas
Daugirdiene, Ausra
Kulikowski, Janus J.
Murray, Ian J.
Chromatic fading following complete adaptation to unique hues
title Chromatic fading following complete adaptation to unique hues
title_full Chromatic fading following complete adaptation to unique hues
title_fullStr Chromatic fading following complete adaptation to unique hues
title_full_unstemmed Chromatic fading following complete adaptation to unique hues
title_short Chromatic fading following complete adaptation to unique hues
title_sort chromatic fading following complete adaptation to unique hues
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7416897/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32589196
http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.6.20
work_keys_str_mv AT stanikunasrytis chromaticfadingfollowingcompleteadaptationtouniquehues
AT kulbokaitevaiva chromaticfadingfollowingcompleteadaptationtouniquehues
AT svegzdaalgimantas chromaticfadingfollowingcompleteadaptationtouniquehues
AT vaitkeviciushenrikas chromaticfadingfollowingcompleteadaptationtouniquehues
AT daugirdieneausra chromaticfadingfollowingcompleteadaptationtouniquehues
AT kulikowskijanusj chromaticfadingfollowingcompleteadaptationtouniquehues
AT murrayianj chromaticfadingfollowingcompleteadaptationtouniquehues