Cargando…

Horizontal–vertical anisotropy with respect to bias and sensitivity

Previous research suggests that participants are biased to assess horizontal distances as smaller than vertical distances. We explored the effect of horizontal versus vertical orientation on the sensitivity, as well as the bias, of distance judgments. In contrast to participants of most past studies...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Dopkins, Stephen, Galyer, Darin
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 2020
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7424113/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32609303
http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.7.1
_version_ 1783570271179898880
author Dopkins, Stephen
Galyer, Darin
author_facet Dopkins, Stephen
Galyer, Darin
author_sort Dopkins, Stephen
collection PubMed
description Previous research suggests that participants are biased to assess horizontal distances as smaller than vertical distances. We explored the effect of horizontal versus vertical orientation on the sensitivity, as well as the bias, of distance judgments. In contrast to participants of most past studies, our participants never directly compared horizontal and vertical distances. On each trial, participants judged the horizontal or vertical distance between a pair of horizontally or vertically oriented dots as being greater than or less than the average distance between previously presented pairs of dots of the same orientation. When the same participants judged horizontal and vertical distance, the distance judged greater than average 50% of the time (P50) was larger for horizontal than for vertical judgments—a pattern consistent with the horizontal–vertical bias of past work. When different participants judged horizontal and vertical distance, the horizontal and vertical P50s did not differ. When participants did not know whether they would judge horizontal or vertical distance on the coming trial, sensitivity was lower for horizontal than vertical judgments. When participants knew what kind of judgment they would make on the coming trial, sensitivity did not differ for horizontal and vertical judgments. We consider the implications of these findings for accounts that attribute the horizontal–vertical bias (1) to real-world-retinal mapping, (2) to the elliptical shape of the visual field, or (3) to the asymmetry of the receptive fields used to assess frontal distance. We suggest that distance field asymmetry must be invoked to explain the present sensitivity effect.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-7424113
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2020
publisher The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-74241132020-08-26 Horizontal–vertical anisotropy with respect to bias and sensitivity Dopkins, Stephen Galyer, Darin J Vis Article Previous research suggests that participants are biased to assess horizontal distances as smaller than vertical distances. We explored the effect of horizontal versus vertical orientation on the sensitivity, as well as the bias, of distance judgments. In contrast to participants of most past studies, our participants never directly compared horizontal and vertical distances. On each trial, participants judged the horizontal or vertical distance between a pair of horizontally or vertically oriented dots as being greater than or less than the average distance between previously presented pairs of dots of the same orientation. When the same participants judged horizontal and vertical distance, the distance judged greater than average 50% of the time (P50) was larger for horizontal than for vertical judgments—a pattern consistent with the horizontal–vertical bias of past work. When different participants judged horizontal and vertical distance, the horizontal and vertical P50s did not differ. When participants did not know whether they would judge horizontal or vertical distance on the coming trial, sensitivity was lower for horizontal than vertical judgments. When participants knew what kind of judgment they would make on the coming trial, sensitivity did not differ for horizontal and vertical judgments. We consider the implications of these findings for accounts that attribute the horizontal–vertical bias (1) to real-world-retinal mapping, (2) to the elliptical shape of the visual field, or (3) to the asymmetry of the receptive fields used to assess frontal distance. We suggest that distance field asymmetry must be invoked to explain the present sensitivity effect. The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 2020-07-01 /pmc/articles/PMC7424113/ /pubmed/32609303 http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.7.1 Text en Copyright 2020 The Authors http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
spellingShingle Article
Dopkins, Stephen
Galyer, Darin
Horizontal–vertical anisotropy with respect to bias and sensitivity
title Horizontal–vertical anisotropy with respect to bias and sensitivity
title_full Horizontal–vertical anisotropy with respect to bias and sensitivity
title_fullStr Horizontal–vertical anisotropy with respect to bias and sensitivity
title_full_unstemmed Horizontal–vertical anisotropy with respect to bias and sensitivity
title_short Horizontal–vertical anisotropy with respect to bias and sensitivity
title_sort horizontal–vertical anisotropy with respect to bias and sensitivity
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7424113/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32609303
http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.7.1
work_keys_str_mv AT dopkinsstephen horizontalverticalanisotropywithrespecttobiasandsensitivity
AT galyerdarin horizontalverticalanisotropywithrespecttobiasandsensitivity