Cargando…
Self-reported impulsivity does not predict response caution()
The broad construct of impulsivity is one that spans both personality and cognitive ability. Despite a common overarching construct, previous research has found no relationship between self-report measures of impulsivity and people's ability to inhibit pre-potent responses. Here, we use evidenc...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Pergamon Press
2020
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7457714/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33273749 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110257 |
_version_ | 1783576052692418560 |
---|---|
author | Hedge, Craig Powell, Georgina Bompas, Aline Sumner, Petroc |
author_facet | Hedge, Craig Powell, Georgina Bompas, Aline Sumner, Petroc |
author_sort | Hedge, Craig |
collection | PubMed |
description | The broad construct of impulsivity is one that spans both personality and cognitive ability. Despite a common overarching construct, previous research has found no relationship between self-report measures of impulsivity and people's ability to inhibit pre-potent responses. Here, we use evidence accumulation models of choice reaction time tasks to extract a measure of “response caution” (boundary separation) and examine whether this correlates with self-reported impulsivity as measured by the UPPS-P questionnaire. Response caution reflects whether an individual makes decisions based on more (favouring accuracy) or less (favouring speed) evidence. We reasoned that this strategic dimension of behaviour is conceptually closer to the tendencies that self-report impulsivity measures probe than what is traditional measured by inhibition tasks. In a meta-analysis of five datasets (total N = 296), encompassing 19 correlations per subscale, we observe no evidence that response caution correlates with self-reported impulsivity. Average correlations between response caution and UPPS-P subscales ranged from rho = −0.02 to −0.04. While the construct of response caution has demonstrated value in understanding individual differences in cognition, brain functioning and aging; the factors underlying what has been called “impulsive information processing” appear to be distinct from the concept of impulsivity derived from self-report. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-7457714 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2020 |
publisher | Pergamon Press |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-74577142020-12-01 Self-reported impulsivity does not predict response caution() Hedge, Craig Powell, Georgina Bompas, Aline Sumner, Petroc Pers Individ Dif Article The broad construct of impulsivity is one that spans both personality and cognitive ability. Despite a common overarching construct, previous research has found no relationship between self-report measures of impulsivity and people's ability to inhibit pre-potent responses. Here, we use evidence accumulation models of choice reaction time tasks to extract a measure of “response caution” (boundary separation) and examine whether this correlates with self-reported impulsivity as measured by the UPPS-P questionnaire. Response caution reflects whether an individual makes decisions based on more (favouring accuracy) or less (favouring speed) evidence. We reasoned that this strategic dimension of behaviour is conceptually closer to the tendencies that self-report impulsivity measures probe than what is traditional measured by inhibition tasks. In a meta-analysis of five datasets (total N = 296), encompassing 19 correlations per subscale, we observe no evidence that response caution correlates with self-reported impulsivity. Average correlations between response caution and UPPS-P subscales ranged from rho = −0.02 to −0.04. While the construct of response caution has demonstrated value in understanding individual differences in cognition, brain functioning and aging; the factors underlying what has been called “impulsive information processing” appear to be distinct from the concept of impulsivity derived from self-report. Pergamon Press 2020-12-01 /pmc/articles/PMC7457714/ /pubmed/33273749 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110257 Text en © 2020 The Authors http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). |
spellingShingle | Article Hedge, Craig Powell, Georgina Bompas, Aline Sumner, Petroc Self-reported impulsivity does not predict response caution() |
title | Self-reported impulsivity does not predict response caution() |
title_full | Self-reported impulsivity does not predict response caution() |
title_fullStr | Self-reported impulsivity does not predict response caution() |
title_full_unstemmed | Self-reported impulsivity does not predict response caution() |
title_short | Self-reported impulsivity does not predict response caution() |
title_sort | self-reported impulsivity does not predict response caution() |
topic | Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7457714/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33273749 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110257 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT hedgecraig selfreportedimpulsivitydoesnotpredictresponsecaution AT powellgeorgina selfreportedimpulsivitydoesnotpredictresponsecaution AT bompasaline selfreportedimpulsivitydoesnotpredictresponsecaution AT sumnerpetroc selfreportedimpulsivitydoesnotpredictresponsecaution |