Cargando…

Naming guides how 12-month-old infants encode and remember objects

A foundation of human cognition is the flexibility with which we can represent any object as either a unique individual (my dog Fred) or a member of an object category (dog, animal). This conceptual flexibility is supported by language; the way we name an object is instrumental to our construal of t...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: LaTourrette, Alexander S., Waxman, Sandra R.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: National Academy of Sciences 2020
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7474636/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32817508
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2006608117
_version_ 1783579364450893824
author LaTourrette, Alexander S.
Waxman, Sandra R.
author_facet LaTourrette, Alexander S.
Waxman, Sandra R.
author_sort LaTourrette, Alexander S.
collection PubMed
description A foundation of human cognition is the flexibility with which we can represent any object as either a unique individual (my dog Fred) or a member of an object category (dog, animal). This conceptual flexibility is supported by language; the way we name an object is instrumental to our construal of that object as an individual or a category member. Evidence from a new recognition memory task reveals that infants are sensitive to this principled link between naming and object representation by age 12 mo. During training, all infants (n = 77) viewed four distinct objects from the same object category, each introduced in conjunction with either the same novel noun (Consistent Name condition), a distinct novel noun for each object (Distinct Names condition), or the same sine-wave tone sequence (Consistent Tone condition). At test, infants saw each training object again, presented in silence along with a new object from the same category. Infants in the Consistent Name condition showed poor recognition memory at test, suggesting that consistently applied names focused them primarily on commonalities among the named objects at the expense of distinctions among them. Infants in the Distinct Names condition recognized three of the four objects, suggesting that applying distinct names enhanced infants’ encoding of the distinctions among the objects. Infants in the control Consistent Tone condition recognized only the object they had most recently seen. Thus, even for infants just beginning to speak their first words, the way in which an object is named guides infants’ encoding, representation, and memory for that object.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-7474636
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2020
publisher National Academy of Sciences
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-74746362020-09-18 Naming guides how 12-month-old infants encode and remember objects LaTourrette, Alexander S. Waxman, Sandra R. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A Social Sciences A foundation of human cognition is the flexibility with which we can represent any object as either a unique individual (my dog Fred) or a member of an object category (dog, animal). This conceptual flexibility is supported by language; the way we name an object is instrumental to our construal of that object as an individual or a category member. Evidence from a new recognition memory task reveals that infants are sensitive to this principled link between naming and object representation by age 12 mo. During training, all infants (n = 77) viewed four distinct objects from the same object category, each introduced in conjunction with either the same novel noun (Consistent Name condition), a distinct novel noun for each object (Distinct Names condition), or the same sine-wave tone sequence (Consistent Tone condition). At test, infants saw each training object again, presented in silence along with a new object from the same category. Infants in the Consistent Name condition showed poor recognition memory at test, suggesting that consistently applied names focused them primarily on commonalities among the named objects at the expense of distinctions among them. Infants in the Distinct Names condition recognized three of the four objects, suggesting that applying distinct names enhanced infants’ encoding of the distinctions among the objects. Infants in the control Consistent Tone condition recognized only the object they had most recently seen. Thus, even for infants just beginning to speak their first words, the way in which an object is named guides infants’ encoding, representation, and memory for that object. National Academy of Sciences 2020-09-01 2020-08-17 /pmc/articles/PMC7474636/ /pubmed/32817508 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2006608117 Text en Copyright © 2020 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/This open access article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND) (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/) .
spellingShingle Social Sciences
LaTourrette, Alexander S.
Waxman, Sandra R.
Naming guides how 12-month-old infants encode and remember objects
title Naming guides how 12-month-old infants encode and remember objects
title_full Naming guides how 12-month-old infants encode and remember objects
title_fullStr Naming guides how 12-month-old infants encode and remember objects
title_full_unstemmed Naming guides how 12-month-old infants encode and remember objects
title_short Naming guides how 12-month-old infants encode and remember objects
title_sort naming guides how 12-month-old infants encode and remember objects
topic Social Sciences
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7474636/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32817508
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2006608117
work_keys_str_mv AT latourrettealexanders namingguideshow12montholdinfantsencodeandrememberobjects
AT waxmansandrar namingguideshow12montholdinfantsencodeandrememberobjects