Cargando…
The role of action intentionality and effector in the subjective expansion of temporal duration after saccadic eye movements
Visual perception is based on periods of stable fixation separated by saccadic eye movements. Although naive perception seems stable (in space) and continuous (in time), laboratory studies have demonstrated that events presented around the time of saccades are misperceived spatially and temporally....
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Nature Publishing Group UK
2020
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7547063/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33037289 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-73830-6 |
_version_ | 1783592354327822336 |
---|---|
author | Melcher, David Kumar, Devpriya Srinivasan, Narayanan |
author_facet | Melcher, David Kumar, Devpriya Srinivasan, Narayanan |
author_sort | Melcher, David |
collection | PubMed |
description | Visual perception is based on periods of stable fixation separated by saccadic eye movements. Although naive perception seems stable (in space) and continuous (in time), laboratory studies have demonstrated that events presented around the time of saccades are misperceived spatially and temporally. Saccadic chronostasis, the “stopped clock illusion”, represents one such temporal distortion in which the movement of the clock hand after the saccade is perceived as lasting longer than usual. Multiple explanations for chronostasis have been proposed including action-backdating, temporal binding of the action towards the moment of its effect (“intentional binding”) and post-saccadic temporal dilation. The current study aimed to resolve this debate by using different types of action (keypress vs saccade) and varying the intentionality of the action. We measured both perceived onset of the motor action and perceived onset of an auditory tone presented at different delays after the keypress/saccade. The results showed intentional binding for the keypress action, with perceived motor onset shifted forwards in time and the time of the tone shifted backwards. Saccades resulted in the opposite pattern, showing temporal expansion rather than compression, especially with cued saccades. The temporal illusion was modulated by intentionality of the movement. Our findings suggest that saccadic chronostasis is not solely dependent on a backward shift in perceived saccade onset, but instead reflects a temporal dilation. This percept of an effectively “longer” period at the beginning of a new fixation may reflect the pattern of suppressed, and then enhanced, visual processing around the time of saccades. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-7547063 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2020 |
publisher | Nature Publishing Group UK |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-75470632020-10-14 The role of action intentionality and effector in the subjective expansion of temporal duration after saccadic eye movements Melcher, David Kumar, Devpriya Srinivasan, Narayanan Sci Rep Article Visual perception is based on periods of stable fixation separated by saccadic eye movements. Although naive perception seems stable (in space) and continuous (in time), laboratory studies have demonstrated that events presented around the time of saccades are misperceived spatially and temporally. Saccadic chronostasis, the “stopped clock illusion”, represents one such temporal distortion in which the movement of the clock hand after the saccade is perceived as lasting longer than usual. Multiple explanations for chronostasis have been proposed including action-backdating, temporal binding of the action towards the moment of its effect (“intentional binding”) and post-saccadic temporal dilation. The current study aimed to resolve this debate by using different types of action (keypress vs saccade) and varying the intentionality of the action. We measured both perceived onset of the motor action and perceived onset of an auditory tone presented at different delays after the keypress/saccade. The results showed intentional binding for the keypress action, with perceived motor onset shifted forwards in time and the time of the tone shifted backwards. Saccades resulted in the opposite pattern, showing temporal expansion rather than compression, especially with cued saccades. The temporal illusion was modulated by intentionality of the movement. Our findings suggest that saccadic chronostasis is not solely dependent on a backward shift in perceived saccade onset, but instead reflects a temporal dilation. This percept of an effectively “longer” period at the beginning of a new fixation may reflect the pattern of suppressed, and then enhanced, visual processing around the time of saccades. Nature Publishing Group UK 2020-10-09 /pmc/articles/PMC7547063/ /pubmed/33037289 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-73830-6 Text en © The Author(s) 2020 Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. |
spellingShingle | Article Melcher, David Kumar, Devpriya Srinivasan, Narayanan The role of action intentionality and effector in the subjective expansion of temporal duration after saccadic eye movements |
title | The role of action intentionality and effector in the subjective expansion of temporal duration after saccadic eye movements |
title_full | The role of action intentionality and effector in the subjective expansion of temporal duration after saccadic eye movements |
title_fullStr | The role of action intentionality and effector in the subjective expansion of temporal duration after saccadic eye movements |
title_full_unstemmed | The role of action intentionality and effector in the subjective expansion of temporal duration after saccadic eye movements |
title_short | The role of action intentionality and effector in the subjective expansion of temporal duration after saccadic eye movements |
title_sort | role of action intentionality and effector in the subjective expansion of temporal duration after saccadic eye movements |
topic | Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7547063/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33037289 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-73830-6 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT melcherdavid theroleofactionintentionalityandeffectorinthesubjectiveexpansionoftemporaldurationaftersaccadiceyemovements AT kumardevpriya theroleofactionintentionalityandeffectorinthesubjectiveexpansionoftemporaldurationaftersaccadiceyemovements AT srinivasannarayanan theroleofactionintentionalityandeffectorinthesubjectiveexpansionoftemporaldurationaftersaccadiceyemovements AT melcherdavid roleofactionintentionalityandeffectorinthesubjectiveexpansionoftemporaldurationaftersaccadiceyemovements AT kumardevpriya roleofactionintentionalityandeffectorinthesubjectiveexpansionoftemporaldurationaftersaccadiceyemovements AT srinivasannarayanan roleofactionintentionalityandeffectorinthesubjectiveexpansionoftemporaldurationaftersaccadiceyemovements |