Cargando…
Motivational factors underlying aversive Pavlovian-instrumental transfer
While interest in active avoidance has recently been resurgent, many concerns relating to the nature of this form of learning remain unresolved. By separating stimulus and response acquisition, aversive Pavlovian-instrumental transfer can be used to measure the effect of avoidance learning on threat...
Autores principales: | , , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press
2020
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7571266/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33060285 http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.052316.120 |
_version_ | 1783597136920707072 |
---|---|
author | Campese, Vinn D. Kim, Ian T. Kurpas, Botagoz Branigan, Lauren Draus, Cassandra LeDoux, Joseph E. |
author_facet | Campese, Vinn D. Kim, Ian T. Kurpas, Botagoz Branigan, Lauren Draus, Cassandra LeDoux, Joseph E. |
author_sort | Campese, Vinn D. |
collection | PubMed |
description | While interest in active avoidance has recently been resurgent, many concerns relating to the nature of this form of learning remain unresolved. By separating stimulus and response acquisition, aversive Pavlovian-instrumental transfer can be used to measure the effect of avoidance learning on threat processing with more control than typical avoidance procedures. However, the motivational substrates that contribute to the aversive transfer effect have not been thoroughly examined. In three studies using rodents, the impact of a variety of aversive signals on shock-avoidance responding (i.e., two-way shuttling) was evaluated. Fox urine, as well as a tone paired with the delivery of the predator odor were insufficient modulatory stimuli for the avoidance response. Similarly, a signal for the absence of food did not generate appropriate aversive motivation to enhance shuttling. Only conditioned Pavlovian stimuli that had been paired with unconditioned threats were capable of augmenting shock-avoidance responding. This was true whether the signaled outcome was the same (e.g., shock) or different (e.g., klaxon) from the avoidance outcome (i.e., shock). These findings help to characterize the aversive transfer effect and provide a more thorough analysis of its generalization to warning signals for different kinds of threats. This feature of aversive motivation has not been demonstrated using conventional avoidance procedures and could be potentially useful for applying avoidance in treatment settings. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-7571266 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2020 |
publisher | Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-75712662021-11-01 Motivational factors underlying aversive Pavlovian-instrumental transfer Campese, Vinn D. Kim, Ian T. Kurpas, Botagoz Branigan, Lauren Draus, Cassandra LeDoux, Joseph E. Learn Mem Research While interest in active avoidance has recently been resurgent, many concerns relating to the nature of this form of learning remain unresolved. By separating stimulus and response acquisition, aversive Pavlovian-instrumental transfer can be used to measure the effect of avoidance learning on threat processing with more control than typical avoidance procedures. However, the motivational substrates that contribute to the aversive transfer effect have not been thoroughly examined. In three studies using rodents, the impact of a variety of aversive signals on shock-avoidance responding (i.e., two-way shuttling) was evaluated. Fox urine, as well as a tone paired with the delivery of the predator odor were insufficient modulatory stimuli for the avoidance response. Similarly, a signal for the absence of food did not generate appropriate aversive motivation to enhance shuttling. Only conditioned Pavlovian stimuli that had been paired with unconditioned threats were capable of augmenting shock-avoidance responding. This was true whether the signaled outcome was the same (e.g., shock) or different (e.g., klaxon) from the avoidance outcome (i.e., shock). These findings help to characterize the aversive transfer effect and provide a more thorough analysis of its generalization to warning signals for different kinds of threats. This feature of aversive motivation has not been demonstrated using conventional avoidance procedures and could be potentially useful for applying avoidance in treatment settings. Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press 2020-11 /pmc/articles/PMC7571266/ /pubmed/33060285 http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.052316.120 Text en © 2020 Campese et al.; Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/ This article is distributed exclusively by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press for the first 12 months after the full-issue publication date (see http://learnmem.cshlp.org/site/misc/terms.xhtml). After 12 months, it is available under a Creative Commons License (Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International), as described at http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/. |
spellingShingle | Research Campese, Vinn D. Kim, Ian T. Kurpas, Botagoz Branigan, Lauren Draus, Cassandra LeDoux, Joseph E. Motivational factors underlying aversive Pavlovian-instrumental transfer |
title | Motivational factors underlying aversive Pavlovian-instrumental transfer |
title_full | Motivational factors underlying aversive Pavlovian-instrumental transfer |
title_fullStr | Motivational factors underlying aversive Pavlovian-instrumental transfer |
title_full_unstemmed | Motivational factors underlying aversive Pavlovian-instrumental transfer |
title_short | Motivational factors underlying aversive Pavlovian-instrumental transfer |
title_sort | motivational factors underlying aversive pavlovian-instrumental transfer |
topic | Research |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7571266/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33060285 http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.052316.120 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT campesevinnd motivationalfactorsunderlyingaversivepavlovianinstrumentaltransfer AT kimiant motivationalfactorsunderlyingaversivepavlovianinstrumentaltransfer AT kurpasbotagoz motivationalfactorsunderlyingaversivepavlovianinstrumentaltransfer AT braniganlauren motivationalfactorsunderlyingaversivepavlovianinstrumentaltransfer AT drauscassandra motivationalfactorsunderlyingaversivepavlovianinstrumentaltransfer AT ledouxjosephe motivationalfactorsunderlyingaversivepavlovianinstrumentaltransfer |