Cargando…

Non-Vocal Behaviors Are More Frequent During the Decisive Negotiation Phases in Barn Owl Siblings

SIMPLE SUMMARY: Animal communication can involve the use of multiple types of signals. While vocal communication has been widely studied in natural populations, there is a dearth of knowledge about the possible role of vibrations or noises made by body movements in communication processes. By using...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: N. Dreiss, Amélie, Romano, Andrea, Flint, Raphaëlle, Bates, Sarah, Vermunt, Aurélie, Henry, Isabelle, A. Ruppli, Charlène, Roulin, Alexandre
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: MDPI 2020
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7601554/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33019525
http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani10101777
_version_ 1783603449914458112
author N. Dreiss, Amélie
Romano, Andrea
Flint, Raphaëlle
Bates, Sarah
Vermunt, Aurélie
Henry, Isabelle
A. Ruppli, Charlène
Roulin, Alexandre
author_facet N. Dreiss, Amélie
Romano, Andrea
Flint, Raphaëlle
Bates, Sarah
Vermunt, Aurélie
Henry, Isabelle
A. Ruppli, Charlène
Roulin, Alexandre
author_sort N. Dreiss, Amélie
collection PubMed
description SIMPLE SUMMARY: Animal communication can involve the use of multiple types of signals. While vocal communication has been widely studied in natural populations, there is a dearth of knowledge about the possible role of vibrations or noises made by body movements in communication processes. By using experimental settings both under natural conditions and in the laboratory, we showed that barn owl nestlings (Tyto alba) produced various non-vocal noises. Movement noises were particularly frequent when nestlings were involved in the intense vocal interactions they use to negotiate the priority for access to the next food item delivered by parents. Body movements might therefore have a role in reinforcing vocal signals during competitive interactions among siblings. ABSTRACT: Animals produce vibrations or noises by means of body movements, which can play a role in communication. These behaviors enhance signal transmission or receiver attention and could be specifically used during turn-taking phases of a reciprocal exchange of signals. In the barn owl Tyto alba, nestlings vocalize one after the other to negotiate which individual will have priority access to the impending prey item to be delivered by the parents. Owlets adjust their vocalization to their own hunger level and to their siblings’ vocalization, withdrawing from the contest in front of highly vocal, and hence hungry, motivated nestmates. As sibling negotiation is a multicomponent display, we examined whether body movements could also be part of the negotiation process. To this end, we analyzed whether the vocalizations of one nestling affected its nestmate’s movements in three separate experiments: in natural nests, in the lab, and using a playback procedure. Nestling barn owls move in a variety of ways, such as repeated tapping of the floor with a foot, scratching the floor with claws, or flapping wings. Body movements were more frequent during the turn-taking phases of vocal interactions, when siblings emitted longer calls and at a greater rate. Once an individual monopolized vocal activity, siblings became less vocal and less active. Moreover, owlets produced more noisy body movements during the phases of vocal interactions which are crucial to prevail in negotiation. Non-vocal physical activities might reinforce vocal signals during sibling to sibling (sib–sib) interactions, or reflect owlets’ arousal, in the critical period during which they vocally settle which individual will dominate the competition.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-7601554
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2020
publisher MDPI
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-76015542020-11-01 Non-Vocal Behaviors Are More Frequent During the Decisive Negotiation Phases in Barn Owl Siblings N. Dreiss, Amélie Romano, Andrea Flint, Raphaëlle Bates, Sarah Vermunt, Aurélie Henry, Isabelle A. Ruppli, Charlène Roulin, Alexandre Animals (Basel) Article SIMPLE SUMMARY: Animal communication can involve the use of multiple types of signals. While vocal communication has been widely studied in natural populations, there is a dearth of knowledge about the possible role of vibrations or noises made by body movements in communication processes. By using experimental settings both under natural conditions and in the laboratory, we showed that barn owl nestlings (Tyto alba) produced various non-vocal noises. Movement noises were particularly frequent when nestlings were involved in the intense vocal interactions they use to negotiate the priority for access to the next food item delivered by parents. Body movements might therefore have a role in reinforcing vocal signals during competitive interactions among siblings. ABSTRACT: Animals produce vibrations or noises by means of body movements, which can play a role in communication. These behaviors enhance signal transmission or receiver attention and could be specifically used during turn-taking phases of a reciprocal exchange of signals. In the barn owl Tyto alba, nestlings vocalize one after the other to negotiate which individual will have priority access to the impending prey item to be delivered by the parents. Owlets adjust their vocalization to their own hunger level and to their siblings’ vocalization, withdrawing from the contest in front of highly vocal, and hence hungry, motivated nestmates. As sibling negotiation is a multicomponent display, we examined whether body movements could also be part of the negotiation process. To this end, we analyzed whether the vocalizations of one nestling affected its nestmate’s movements in three separate experiments: in natural nests, in the lab, and using a playback procedure. Nestling barn owls move in a variety of ways, such as repeated tapping of the floor with a foot, scratching the floor with claws, or flapping wings. Body movements were more frequent during the turn-taking phases of vocal interactions, when siblings emitted longer calls and at a greater rate. Once an individual monopolized vocal activity, siblings became less vocal and less active. Moreover, owlets produced more noisy body movements during the phases of vocal interactions which are crucial to prevail in negotiation. Non-vocal physical activities might reinforce vocal signals during sibling to sibling (sib–sib) interactions, or reflect owlets’ arousal, in the critical period during which they vocally settle which individual will dominate the competition. MDPI 2020-10-01 /pmc/articles/PMC7601554/ /pubmed/33019525 http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani10101777 Text en © 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
spellingShingle Article
N. Dreiss, Amélie
Romano, Andrea
Flint, Raphaëlle
Bates, Sarah
Vermunt, Aurélie
Henry, Isabelle
A. Ruppli, Charlène
Roulin, Alexandre
Non-Vocal Behaviors Are More Frequent During the Decisive Negotiation Phases in Barn Owl Siblings
title Non-Vocal Behaviors Are More Frequent During the Decisive Negotiation Phases in Barn Owl Siblings
title_full Non-Vocal Behaviors Are More Frequent During the Decisive Negotiation Phases in Barn Owl Siblings
title_fullStr Non-Vocal Behaviors Are More Frequent During the Decisive Negotiation Phases in Barn Owl Siblings
title_full_unstemmed Non-Vocal Behaviors Are More Frequent During the Decisive Negotiation Phases in Barn Owl Siblings
title_short Non-Vocal Behaviors Are More Frequent During the Decisive Negotiation Phases in Barn Owl Siblings
title_sort non-vocal behaviors are more frequent during the decisive negotiation phases in barn owl siblings
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7601554/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33019525
http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani10101777
work_keys_str_mv AT ndreissamelie nonvocalbehaviorsaremorefrequentduringthedecisivenegotiationphasesinbarnowlsiblings
AT romanoandrea nonvocalbehaviorsaremorefrequentduringthedecisivenegotiationphasesinbarnowlsiblings
AT flintraphaelle nonvocalbehaviorsaremorefrequentduringthedecisivenegotiationphasesinbarnowlsiblings
AT batessarah nonvocalbehaviorsaremorefrequentduringthedecisivenegotiationphasesinbarnowlsiblings
AT vermuntaurelie nonvocalbehaviorsaremorefrequentduringthedecisivenegotiationphasesinbarnowlsiblings
AT henryisabelle nonvocalbehaviorsaremorefrequentduringthedecisivenegotiationphasesinbarnowlsiblings
AT arupplicharlene nonvocalbehaviorsaremorefrequentduringthedecisivenegotiationphasesinbarnowlsiblings
AT roulinalexandre nonvocalbehaviorsaremorefrequentduringthedecisivenegotiationphasesinbarnowlsiblings