Cargando…

Measures of Listening Effort Are Multidimensional

Listening effort can be defined as the cognitive resources required to perform a listening task. The literature on listening effort is as confusing as it is voluminous: measures of listening effort rarely correlate with each other and sometimes result in contradictory findings. Here, we directly com...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Alhanbali, Sara, Dawes, Piers, Millman, Rebecca E., Munro, Kevin J.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Williams And Wilkins 2019
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7664710/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30747742
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0000000000000697
_version_ 1783609889826799616
author Alhanbali, Sara
Dawes, Piers
Millman, Rebecca E.
Munro, Kevin J.
author_facet Alhanbali, Sara
Dawes, Piers
Millman, Rebecca E.
Munro, Kevin J.
author_sort Alhanbali, Sara
collection PubMed
description Listening effort can be defined as the cognitive resources required to perform a listening task. The literature on listening effort is as confusing as it is voluminous: measures of listening effort rarely correlate with each other and sometimes result in contradictory findings. Here, we directly compared simultaneously recorded multimodal measures of listening effort. After establishing the reliability of the measures, we investigated validity by quantifying correlations between measures and then grouping-related measures through factor analysis. DESIGN: One hundred and sixteen participants with audiometric thresholds ranging from normal to severe hearing loss took part in the study (age range: 55 to 85 years old, 50.3% male). We simultaneously measured pupil size, electroencephalographic alpha power, skin conductance, and self-report listening effort. One self-report measure of fatigue was also included. The signal to noise ratio (SNR) was adjusted at 71% criterion performance using sequences of 3 digits. The main listening task involved correct recall of a random digit from a sequence of six presented at a SNR where performance was around 82 to 93%. Test–retest reliability of the measures was established by retesting 30 participants 7 days after the initial session. RESULTS: With the exception of skin conductance and the self-report measure of fatigue, interclass correlation coefficients (ICC) revealed good test–retest reliability (minimum ICC: 0.71). Weak or nonsignificant correlations were identified between measures. Factor analysis, using only the reliable measures, revealed four underlying dimensions: factor 1 included SNR, hearing level, baseline alpha power, and performance accuracy; factor 2 included pupillometry; factor 3 included alpha power (during speech presentation and during retention); factor 4 included self-reported listening effort and baseline alpha power. CONCLUSIONS: The good ICC suggests that poor test reliability is not the reason for the lack of correlation between measures. We have demonstrated that measures traditionally used as indicators of listening effort tap into multiple underlying dimensions. We therefore propose that there is no “gold standard” measure of listening effort and that different measures of listening effort should not be used interchangeably. When choosing method(s) to measure listening effort, the nature of the task and aspects of increased listening demands that are of interest should be taken into account. The findings of this study provide a framework for understanding and interpreting listening effort measures.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-7664710
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2019
publisher Williams And Wilkins
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-76647102020-11-16 Measures of Listening Effort Are Multidimensional Alhanbali, Sara Dawes, Piers Millman, Rebecca E. Munro, Kevin J. Ear Hear Research Articles Listening effort can be defined as the cognitive resources required to perform a listening task. The literature on listening effort is as confusing as it is voluminous: measures of listening effort rarely correlate with each other and sometimes result in contradictory findings. Here, we directly compared simultaneously recorded multimodal measures of listening effort. After establishing the reliability of the measures, we investigated validity by quantifying correlations between measures and then grouping-related measures through factor analysis. DESIGN: One hundred and sixteen participants with audiometric thresholds ranging from normal to severe hearing loss took part in the study (age range: 55 to 85 years old, 50.3% male). We simultaneously measured pupil size, electroencephalographic alpha power, skin conductance, and self-report listening effort. One self-report measure of fatigue was also included. The signal to noise ratio (SNR) was adjusted at 71% criterion performance using sequences of 3 digits. The main listening task involved correct recall of a random digit from a sequence of six presented at a SNR where performance was around 82 to 93%. Test–retest reliability of the measures was established by retesting 30 participants 7 days after the initial session. RESULTS: With the exception of skin conductance and the self-report measure of fatigue, interclass correlation coefficients (ICC) revealed good test–retest reliability (minimum ICC: 0.71). Weak or nonsignificant correlations were identified between measures. Factor analysis, using only the reliable measures, revealed four underlying dimensions: factor 1 included SNR, hearing level, baseline alpha power, and performance accuracy; factor 2 included pupillometry; factor 3 included alpha power (during speech presentation and during retention); factor 4 included self-reported listening effort and baseline alpha power. CONCLUSIONS: The good ICC suggests that poor test reliability is not the reason for the lack of correlation between measures. We have demonstrated that measures traditionally used as indicators of listening effort tap into multiple underlying dimensions. We therefore propose that there is no “gold standard” measure of listening effort and that different measures of listening effort should not be used interchangeably. When choosing method(s) to measure listening effort, the nature of the task and aspects of increased listening demands that are of interest should be taken into account. The findings of this study provide a framework for understanding and interpreting listening effort measures. Williams And Wilkins 2019-08-23 /pmc/articles/PMC7664710/ /pubmed/30747742 http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0000000000000697 Text en Copyright © 2019 The Authors. Ear & Hearing is published on behalf of the American Auditory Society, by Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License 4.0 (CCBY) (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
spellingShingle Research Articles
Alhanbali, Sara
Dawes, Piers
Millman, Rebecca E.
Munro, Kevin J.
Measures of Listening Effort Are Multidimensional
title Measures of Listening Effort Are Multidimensional
title_full Measures of Listening Effort Are Multidimensional
title_fullStr Measures of Listening Effort Are Multidimensional
title_full_unstemmed Measures of Listening Effort Are Multidimensional
title_short Measures of Listening Effort Are Multidimensional
title_sort measures of listening effort are multidimensional
topic Research Articles
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7664710/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30747742
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0000000000000697
work_keys_str_mv AT alhanbalisara measuresoflisteningeffortaremultidimensional
AT dawespiers measuresoflisteningeffortaremultidimensional
AT millmanrebeccae measuresoflisteningeffortaremultidimensional
AT munrokevinj measuresoflisteningeffortaremultidimensional