Cargando…
Are New Gender-Neutral Pronouns Difficult to Process in Reading? The Case of Hen in SWEDISH
Hen is a Swedish gender-neutral pronoun used for non-binary individuals and as a generic singular pronoun form. Hen was added to the Swedish Academy Glossary (SAOL) in 2015, and opponents of hen have argued that gender-neutral pronouns are difficult to process, and therefore should not be used. As o...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2020
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7683789/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33244303 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.574356 |
_version_ | 1783612954357268480 |
---|---|
author | Vergoossen, Hellen P. Pärnamets, Philip Renström, Emma A. Gustafsson Sendén, Marie |
author_facet | Vergoossen, Hellen P. Pärnamets, Philip Renström, Emma A. Gustafsson Sendén, Marie |
author_sort | Vergoossen, Hellen P. |
collection | PubMed |
description | Hen is a Swedish gender-neutral pronoun used for non-binary individuals and as a generic singular pronoun form. Hen was added to the Swedish Academy Glossary (SAOL) in 2015, and opponents of hen have argued that gender-neutral pronouns are difficult to process, and therefore should not be used. As of yet, this has not been empirically tested. This pre-registered study used eye-tracking to experimentally test if hen has a processing cost by measuring the process of understanding whom a pronoun refers to (i.e., pronoun resolution). Participants (N = 120) read 48 sentence pairs where the first sentence included a noun referring to a person (e.g., sister, hairdresser, person) and the second included a pronoun referring to the noun. The pronouns were either gendered (she and he) or gender-neutral (hen). The nouns were either neutral (e.g., person, colleague) or gendered, either by lexically referring to gender (e.g., sister, king), or by being associated with stereotypes based on occupational gender segregation (e.g., occupational titles like hairdresser, carpenter). We tested if hen had a greater processing cost than gendered pronouns, and whether the type of noun moderated this effect. The hypotheses were that hen referring to neutral nouns would lead to a smaller processing cost than hen referring to gendered nouns. Furthermore, we hypothesized that hen referring to lexically gendered nouns would lead to larger processing costs than stereotypically gendered role nouns. The processing cost of hen was measured by reading time spent on three regions of the sentence pairs; the pronoun, the spillover region (i.e., the words following the pronoun), and the noun. The only processing cost for hen occurred in the spillover region. The processing cost in this region was greater when hen referred to neutral nouns than when hen referred to a noun associated with gender. In contrast to the hypothesis, the type of gender information associated with the noun did not interact with these effects (i.e., the same reading time for hen following e.g., the queen or carpenter). Altogether, the results do not support that gender-neutral pronouns should be avoided because they are difficult to process. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-7683789 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2020 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-76837892020-11-25 Are New Gender-Neutral Pronouns Difficult to Process in Reading? The Case of Hen in SWEDISH Vergoossen, Hellen P. Pärnamets, Philip Renström, Emma A. Gustafsson Sendén, Marie Front Psychol Psychology Hen is a Swedish gender-neutral pronoun used for non-binary individuals and as a generic singular pronoun form. Hen was added to the Swedish Academy Glossary (SAOL) in 2015, and opponents of hen have argued that gender-neutral pronouns are difficult to process, and therefore should not be used. As of yet, this has not been empirically tested. This pre-registered study used eye-tracking to experimentally test if hen has a processing cost by measuring the process of understanding whom a pronoun refers to (i.e., pronoun resolution). Participants (N = 120) read 48 sentence pairs where the first sentence included a noun referring to a person (e.g., sister, hairdresser, person) and the second included a pronoun referring to the noun. The pronouns were either gendered (she and he) or gender-neutral (hen). The nouns were either neutral (e.g., person, colleague) or gendered, either by lexically referring to gender (e.g., sister, king), or by being associated with stereotypes based on occupational gender segregation (e.g., occupational titles like hairdresser, carpenter). We tested if hen had a greater processing cost than gendered pronouns, and whether the type of noun moderated this effect. The hypotheses were that hen referring to neutral nouns would lead to a smaller processing cost than hen referring to gendered nouns. Furthermore, we hypothesized that hen referring to lexically gendered nouns would lead to larger processing costs than stereotypically gendered role nouns. The processing cost of hen was measured by reading time spent on three regions of the sentence pairs; the pronoun, the spillover region (i.e., the words following the pronoun), and the noun. The only processing cost for hen occurred in the spillover region. The processing cost in this region was greater when hen referred to neutral nouns than when hen referred to a noun associated with gender. In contrast to the hypothesis, the type of gender information associated with the noun did not interact with these effects (i.e., the same reading time for hen following e.g., the queen or carpenter). Altogether, the results do not support that gender-neutral pronouns should be avoided because they are difficult to process. Frontiers Media S.A. 2020-11-10 /pmc/articles/PMC7683789/ /pubmed/33244303 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.574356 Text en Copyright © 2020 Vergoossen, Pärnamets, Renström and Gustafsson Sendén. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Psychology Vergoossen, Hellen P. Pärnamets, Philip Renström, Emma A. Gustafsson Sendén, Marie Are New Gender-Neutral Pronouns Difficult to Process in Reading? The Case of Hen in SWEDISH |
title | Are New Gender-Neutral Pronouns Difficult to Process in Reading? The Case of Hen in SWEDISH |
title_full | Are New Gender-Neutral Pronouns Difficult to Process in Reading? The Case of Hen in SWEDISH |
title_fullStr | Are New Gender-Neutral Pronouns Difficult to Process in Reading? The Case of Hen in SWEDISH |
title_full_unstemmed | Are New Gender-Neutral Pronouns Difficult to Process in Reading? The Case of Hen in SWEDISH |
title_short | Are New Gender-Neutral Pronouns Difficult to Process in Reading? The Case of Hen in SWEDISH |
title_sort | are new gender-neutral pronouns difficult to process in reading? the case of hen in swedish |
topic | Psychology |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7683789/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33244303 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.574356 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT vergoossenhellenp arenewgenderneutralpronounsdifficulttoprocessinreadingthecaseofheninswedish AT parnametsphilip arenewgenderneutralpronounsdifficulttoprocessinreadingthecaseofheninswedish AT renstromemmaa arenewgenderneutralpronounsdifficulttoprocessinreadingthecaseofheninswedish AT gustafssonsendenmarie arenewgenderneutralpronounsdifficulttoprocessinreadingthecaseofheninswedish |