Cargando…
Exploitative leaders incite intergroup warfare in a social mammal
Collective conflicts among humans are widespread, although often highly destructive. A classic explanation for the prevalence of such warfare in some human societies is leadership by self-serving individuals that reap the benefits of conflict while other members of society pay the costs. Here, we sh...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
National Academy of Sciences
2020
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7703641/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33168743 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2003745117 |
_version_ | 1783616671416582144 |
---|---|
author | Johnstone, Rufus A. Cant, Michael A. Cram, Dominic Thompson, Faye J. |
author_facet | Johnstone, Rufus A. Cant, Michael A. Cram, Dominic Thompson, Faye J. |
author_sort | Johnstone, Rufus A. |
collection | PubMed |
description | Collective conflicts among humans are widespread, although often highly destructive. A classic explanation for the prevalence of such warfare in some human societies is leadership by self-serving individuals that reap the benefits of conflict while other members of society pay the costs. Here, we show that leadership of this kind can also explain the evolution of collective violence in certain animal societies. We first extend the classic hawk−dove model of the evolution of animal aggression to consider cases in which a subset of individuals within each group may initiate fights in which all group members become involved. We show that leadership of this kind, when combined with inequalities in the payoffs of fighting, can lead to the evolution of severe intergroup aggression, with negative consequences for population mean fitness. We test our model using long-term data from wild banded mongooses, a species characterized by frequent intergroup conflicts that have very different fitness consequences for male and female group members. The data show that aggressive encounters between groups are initiated by females, who gain fitness benefits from mating with extragroup males in the midst of battle, whereas the costs of fighting are borne chiefly by males. In line with the model predictions, the result is unusually severe levels of intergroup violence. Our findings suggest that the decoupling of leaders from the costs that they incite amplifies the destructive nature of intergroup conflict. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-7703641 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2020 |
publisher | National Academy of Sciences |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-77036412020-12-10 Exploitative leaders incite intergroup warfare in a social mammal Johnstone, Rufus A. Cant, Michael A. Cram, Dominic Thompson, Faye J. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A Biological Sciences Collective conflicts among humans are widespread, although often highly destructive. A classic explanation for the prevalence of such warfare in some human societies is leadership by self-serving individuals that reap the benefits of conflict while other members of society pay the costs. Here, we show that leadership of this kind can also explain the evolution of collective violence in certain animal societies. We first extend the classic hawk−dove model of the evolution of animal aggression to consider cases in which a subset of individuals within each group may initiate fights in which all group members become involved. We show that leadership of this kind, when combined with inequalities in the payoffs of fighting, can lead to the evolution of severe intergroup aggression, with negative consequences for population mean fitness. We test our model using long-term data from wild banded mongooses, a species characterized by frequent intergroup conflicts that have very different fitness consequences for male and female group members. The data show that aggressive encounters between groups are initiated by females, who gain fitness benefits from mating with extragroup males in the midst of battle, whereas the costs of fighting are borne chiefly by males. In line with the model predictions, the result is unusually severe levels of intergroup violence. Our findings suggest that the decoupling of leaders from the costs that they incite amplifies the destructive nature of intergroup conflict. National Academy of Sciences 2020-11-24 2020-11-09 /pmc/articles/PMC7703641/ /pubmed/33168743 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2003745117 Text en Copyright © 2020 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/This open access article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND) (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/) . |
spellingShingle | Biological Sciences Johnstone, Rufus A. Cant, Michael A. Cram, Dominic Thompson, Faye J. Exploitative leaders incite intergroup warfare in a social mammal |
title | Exploitative leaders incite intergroup warfare in a social mammal |
title_full | Exploitative leaders incite intergroup warfare in a social mammal |
title_fullStr | Exploitative leaders incite intergroup warfare in a social mammal |
title_full_unstemmed | Exploitative leaders incite intergroup warfare in a social mammal |
title_short | Exploitative leaders incite intergroup warfare in a social mammal |
title_sort | exploitative leaders incite intergroup warfare in a social mammal |
topic | Biological Sciences |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7703641/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33168743 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2003745117 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT johnstonerufusa exploitativeleadersinciteintergroupwarfareinasocialmammal AT cantmichaela exploitativeleadersinciteintergroupwarfareinasocialmammal AT cramdominic exploitativeleadersinciteintergroupwarfareinasocialmammal AT thompsonfayej exploitativeleadersinciteintergroupwarfareinasocialmammal |